Cao J, Luo J, Zhou J, Jiang Y
Front Hum Neurosci. 2024; 18:1402746.
PMID: 38983754
PMC: 11231079.
DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2024.1402746.
Dufour R, Breton E, Morin A, Cote S, Dubois L, Vitaro F
J Eat Disord. 2023; 11(1):183.
PMID: 37833803
PMC: 10571422.
DOI: 10.1186/s40337-023-00902-z.
Guler K, Aydin A
Dement Geriatr Cogn Dis Extra. 2022; 12(3):131-149.
PMID: 36465839
PMC: 9710454.
DOI: 10.1159/000526226.
Nie P, Wang C, Rong G, Du B, Lu J, Li S
Front Psychol. 2022; 12:770425.
PMID: 35153898
PMC: 8825862.
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.770425.
Faerman A, Spiegel D
Sci Rep. 2021; 11(1):5704.
PMID: 33707531
PMC: 7970985.
DOI: 10.1038/s41598-021-84954-8.
A Developmental Eye Tracking Investigation of Cued Task Switching Performance.
Zheng A, Church J
Child Dev. 2021; 92(4):1652-1672.
PMID: 33417266
PMC: 8451801.
DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13478.
Selectively Enhanced Development of Working Memory in Musically Trained Children and Adolescents.
Saarikivi K, Huotilainen M, Tervaniemi M, Putkinen V
Front Integr Neurosci. 2019; 13:62.
PMID: 31780907
PMC: 6851266.
DOI: 10.3389/fnint.2019.00062.
Temporal dynamics of spontaneous default-mode network activity mediate the association between reappraisal and depression.
Gao W, Chen S, Biswal B, Lei X, Yuan J
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2018; 13(12):1235-1247.
PMID: 30339260
PMC: 6277739.
DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsy092.
Consistent Performance Differences between Children and Adults Despite Manipulation of Cue-Target Variables.
Bauer J, Martinez J, Roe M, Church J
Front Psychol. 2017; 8:1304.
PMID: 28824489
PMC: 5541061.
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01304.
Sustained selective attention predicts flexible switching in preschoolers.
Benitez V, Vales C, Hanania R, Smith L
J Exp Child Psychol. 2016; 156:29-42.
PMID: 28024178
PMC: 5253114.
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.11.004.
Flexibility in task switching by monolinguals and bilinguals.
Wiseheart M, Viswanathan M, Bialystok E
Biling (Camb Engl). 2016; 19(1):141-146.
PMID: 26877705
PMC: 4749032.
DOI: 10.1017/S1366728914000273.
Different Executive Functions Support Different Kinds of Cognitive Flexibility: Evidence From 2-, 3-, and 4-Year-Olds.
Blakey E, Visser I, Carroll D
Child Dev. 2015; 87(2):513-26.
PMID: 26659697
PMC: 4991299.
DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12468.
A developmental window into trade-offs in executive function: the case of task switching versus response inhibition in 6-year-olds.
Blackwell K, Chatham C, Wiseheart M, Munakata Y
Neuropsychologia. 2014; 62:356-64.
PMID: 24791710
PMC: 4167192.
DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.04.016.
Why won't you do what I want? The informative failures of children and models.
Chatham C, Yerys B, Munakata Y
Cogn Dev. 2014; 27(4):349-366.
PMID: 24453404
PMC: 3894146.
DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2012.07.003.
Stuck in the moment: cognitive inflexibility in preschoolers following an extended time period.
Garcia C, Dick A
Front Psychol. 2014; 4:959.
PMID: 24399978
PMC: 3872041.
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00959.
Costs and benefits linked to developments in cognitive control.
Blackwell K, Munakata Y
Dev Sci. 2013; 17(2):203-11.
PMID: 24329774
PMC: 4078978.
DOI: 10.1111/desc.12113.
So many options, so little control: abstract representations can reduce selection demands to increase children's self-directed flexibility.
Snyder H, Munakata Y
J Exp Child Psychol. 2013; 116(3):659-73.
PMID: 23998951
PMC: 3778917.
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.07.010.
Speed isn't everything: complex processing speed measures mask individual differences and developmental changes in executive control.
Cepeda N, Blackwell K, Munakata Y
Dev Sci. 2013; 16(2):269-286.
PMID: 23432836
PMC: 3582037.
DOI: 10.1111/desc.12024.
Developing Cognitive Control: Three Key Transitions.
Munakata Y, Snyder H, Chatham C
Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2012; 21(2):71-77.
PMID: 22711982
PMC: 3375849.
DOI: 10.1177/0963721412436807.
When seeing is knowing: the role of visual cues in the dissociation between children's rule knowledge and rule use.
Buss A, Spencer J
J Exp Child Psychol. 2011; 111(3):561-9.
PMID: 22176927
PMC: 3253883.
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.11.005.