» Articles » PMID: 18229475

Why Do Children Perseverate when They Seem to Know Better: Graded Working Memory, or Directed Inhibition?

Overview
Specialty Psychology
Date 2008 Jan 31
PMID 18229475
Citations 13
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Children sometimes have trouble switching from one task to another, despite demonstrating an awareness of current task demands. This behavior could reflect problems either directly inhibiting previously relevant information or sufficiently activating graded working me mory representations forthe current task. We tested competing predictions from each account, using a computerized card-sorting task in which we assessed children's task switching abilities and their response speed to simple questions about current task demands. All children answered these questions correctly, but children who successfully switched tasks responded more quickly to questions than did children who perseverated on previous tasks, even after factoring out processing speed and age. This reaction time difference supports graded working memory accounts, with stronger representations of current task demands aiding both task-switching and responses to questions. This result poses a challenge for directed inhibition accounts, because nothing needs to be inhibited to answer simple questions that lack conflicting information.

Citing Articles

Relationship between physical fitness and executive function in preschool children: a cross-sectional study.

Zhou Z, Chen Y, Huang K, Zeng F, Liang Z, Wang N BMC Sports Sci Med Rehabil. 2024; 16(1):238.

PMID: 39633414 PMC: 11616152. DOI: 10.1186/s13102-024-01028-8.


Emotional dampening in hypertension: Impaired recognition of implicit emotional content in auditory and cross-modal stimuli.

Shukla M, Pandey R Psych J. 2023; 13(1):124-138.

PMID: 37942991 PMC: 10917100. DOI: 10.1002/pchj.704.


"Look! It is not a bamoule!": 18- and 24-month-olds can use negative sentences to constrain their interpretation of novel word meanings.

DE Carvalho A, Crimon C, Barrault A, Trueswell J, Christophe A Dev Sci. 2021; 24(4):e13085.

PMID: 33484223 PMC: 8282655. DOI: 10.1111/desc.13085.


A Developmental Eye Tracking Investigation of Cued Task Switching Performance.

Zheng A, Church J Child Dev. 2021; 92(4):1652-1672.

PMID: 33417266 PMC: 8451801. DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13478.


Neural Correlates of Set-Shifting in Children With Autism.

Yerys B, Antezana L, Weinblatt R, Jankowski K, Strang J, Vaidya C Autism Res. 2015; 8(4):386-97.

PMID: 25599972 PMC: 4508240. DOI: 10.1002/aur.1454.


References
1.
Mayr U . Inhibition of action rules. Psychon Bull Rev. 2002; 9(1):93-9. DOI: 10.3758/bf03196261. View

2.
Cepeda N, Kramer A, Gonzalez de Sather J . Changes in executive control across the life span: examination of task-switching performance. Dev Psychol. 2001; 37(5):715-30. View

3.
Yerys B, Munakata Y . When labels hurt but novelty helps: children's perseveration and flexibility in a card-sorting task. Child Dev. 2006; 77(6):1589-607. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00961.x. View

4.
Noble K, Norman M, Farah M . Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Dev Sci. 2005; 8(1):74-87. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00394.x. View

5.
Tsuchiya E, Oki J, Yahara N, Fujieda K . Computerized version of the Wisconsin card sorting test in children with high-functioning autistic disorder or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Brain Dev. 2005; 27(3):233-6. DOI: 10.1016/j.braindev.2004.06.008. View