Wan C, Cai H, Li F
Behav Sci (Basel). 2024; 14(7).
PMID: 39062401
PMC: 11274188.
DOI: 10.3390/bs14070578.
Munakata Y, Michaelson L
Annu Rev Dev Psychol. 2024; 3(1):139-163.
PMID: 38993653
PMC: 11238700.
DOI: 10.1146/annurev-devpsych-121318-085005.
Fitamen C, Camos V
Front Psychol. 2021; 12:659020.
PMID: 33995220
PMC: 8113615.
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.659020.
Fitamen C, Blaye A, Camos V
Sci Rep. 2019; 9(1):15342.
PMID: 31653944
PMC: 6814763.
DOI: 10.1038/s41598-019-51869-4.
Oeri N, Buttelmann D, Voelke A, Roebers C
Front Psychol. 2019; 10:977.
PMID: 31133927
PMC: 6517522.
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00977.
The Cost of Prospective Memory in Children: The Role of Cue Focality.
Cejudo A, Gomez-Ariza C, Bajo M
Front Psychol. 2019; 9:2738.
PMID: 30687189
PMC: 6333704.
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02738.
Getting ready to use control: Advances in the measurement of young children's use of proactive control.
Doebel S, Barker J, Chevalier N, Michaelson L, Fisher A, Munakata Y
PLoS One. 2017; 12(4):e0175072.
PMID: 28419099
PMC: 5395143.
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0175072.
A meta-analysis of the Dimensional Change Card Sort: Implications for developmental theories and the measurement of executive function in children.
Doebel S, Zelazo P
Dev Rev. 2016; 38:241-268.
PMID: 26955206
PMC: 4778090.
DOI: 10.1016/j.dr.2015.09.001.
Impact of Reminders on Children's Cognitive Flexibility, Intrinsic Motivation, and Mood Depends on Who Provides the Reminders.
Qu L, Ong J
Front Psychol. 2016; 6:1904.
PMID: 26779058
PMC: 4700142.
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01904.
Event structure and cognitive control.
Reimer J, Radvansky G, Lorsbach T, Armendarez J
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2015; 41(5):1374-87.
PMID: 25603168
PMC: 5718630.
DOI: 10.1037/xlm0000105.
Age-related changes in the temporal dynamics of executive control: a study in 5- and 6-year-old children.
Lucenet J, Blaye A
Front Psychol. 2014; 5:831.
PMID: 25120523
PMC: 4114259.
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00831.
Cognitive flexibility predicts early reading skills.
Cole P, Duncan L, Blaye A
Front Psychol. 2014; 5:565.
PMID: 24966842
PMC: 4052802.
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00565.
A developmental window into trade-offs in executive function: the case of task switching versus response inhibition in 6-year-olds.
Blackwell K, Chatham C, Wiseheart M, Munakata Y
Neuropsychologia. 2014; 62:356-64.
PMID: 24791710
PMC: 4167192.
DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.04.016.
So many options, so little control: abstract representations can reduce selection demands to increase children's self-directed flexibility.
Snyder H, Munakata Y
J Exp Child Psychol. 2013; 116(3):659-73.
PMID: 23998951
PMC: 3778917.
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.07.010.
Underpinnings of the costs of flexibility in preschool children: the roles of inhibition and working memory.
Chevalier N, Sheffield T, Nelson J, Clark C, Wiebe S, Espy K
Dev Neuropsychol. 2012; 37(2):99-118.
PMID: 22339225
PMC: 3308575.
DOI: 10.1080/87565641.2011.632458.
The Role of Representations in Executive Function: Investigating a Developmental Link between Flexibility and Abstraction.
Kharitonova M, Munakata Y
Front Psychol. 2011; 2:347.
PMID: 22144971
PMC: 3227021.
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00347.
Switch detection in preschoolers' cognitive flexibility.
Chevalier N, Wiebe S, Huber K, Espy K
J Exp Child Psychol. 2011; 109(3):353-70.
PMID: 21353678
PMC: 3065531.
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.01.006.
Becoming self-directed: abstract representations support endogenous flexibility in children.
Snyder H, Munakata Y
Cognition. 2010; 116(2):155-67.
PMID: 20472227
PMC: 2900525.
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.04.007.
When simple things are meaningful: working memory strength predicts children's cognitive flexibility.
Blackwell K, Cepeda N, Munakata Y
J Exp Child Psychol. 2009; 103(2):241-9.
PMID: 19278688
PMC: 2737814.
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.01.002.