» Articles » PMID: 12956243

The Nonindependence of Stimulus Properties in Human Category Learning

Overview
Journal Mem Cognit
Specialty Psychology
Date 2003 Sep 6
PMID 12956243
Citations 11
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Typically, models of category learning are verified through behavioral experiments with stimuli consisting of putatively independent dimensions such as shape, size, and color. The assumption of independence is critical in both the design of behavioral experiments and the development of models and theories of learning. Using the standard classification learning paradigm and a common stimulus set, the present work demonstrates that the assumption of independence is unwarranted. Systematic relations span stimulus dimensions and govern learning performance. For example, shape is not independent of size and color, because humans quantify size and color over shape when shape is relevant to the categorization. This quantification is reflected in natural language use (e.g., "blue triangle" as opposed to "triangle and blue"). In this example, color and size are predicates and shape is the argument. Across four experiments, the difficulty of mastering a classification rule can be predicted by the number of predicates that must be unbound in order to free rule-relevant stimulus dimensions.

Citing Articles

A boundedly rational model for category learning.

Houser T Front Psychol. 2024; 15:1477514.

PMID: 39717468 PMC: 11663663. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1477514.


REFRESH: A new approach to modeling dimensional biases in perceptual similarity and categorization.

Sanborn A, Heller K, Austerweil J, Chater N Psychol Rev. 2021; 128(6):1145-1186.

PMID: 34516151 PMC: 8567459. DOI: 10.1037/rev0000310.


Simple and Complex Working Memory Tasks Allow Similar Benefits of Information Compression.

Mathy F, Chekaf M, Cowan N J Cogn. 2019; 1(1):31.

PMID: 31517204 PMC: 6634419. DOI: 10.5334/joc.31.


Classification errors and response times over multiple distributed sessions as a function of category structure.

Zeigler D, Vigo R Mem Cognit. 2018; 46(7):1041-1057.

PMID: 29752660 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-018-0820-x.


Determining transformation distance in similarity: Considerations for assessing representational changes a priori.

Grimm L, Rein J, Markman A Think Reason. 2016; 18(1):59-80.

PMID: 27642252 PMC: 5026241. DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2011.649094.


References
1.
Kruschke J . ALCOVE: an exemplar-based connectionist model of category learning. Psychol Rev. 1992; 99(1):22-44. DOI: 10.1037/0033-295x.99.1.22. View

2.
Yamauchi T, Love B, Markman A . Learning nonlinearly separable categories by inference and classification. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2002; 28(3):585-93. DOI: 10.1037//0278-7393.28.3.585. View

3.
Nosofsky R, Palmeri T, McKinley S . Rule-plus-exception model of classification learning. Psychol Rev. 1994; 101(1):53-79. DOI: 10.1037/0033-295x.101.1.53. View

4.
Marks L . On cross-modal similarity: the perceptual structure of pitch, loudness, and brightness. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1989; 15(3):586-602. DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.15.3.586. View

5.
Markman A, Ross B . Category use and category learning. Psychol Bull. 2003; 129(4):592-613. DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.4.592. View