» Articles » PMID: 35405421

Distinct but Correlated Latent Factors Support the Regulation of Learned Conflict-control and Task-switching

Overview
Journal Cogn Psychol
Publisher Elsevier
Specialty Psychology
Date 2022 Apr 11
PMID 35405421
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Cognitive control is guided by learning, as people adjust control to meet changing task demands. The two best-studied instances of "control-learning" are the enhancement of attentional task focus in response to increased frequencies of incongruent distracter stimuli, reflected in the list-wide proportion congruent (LWPC) effect, and the enhancement of switch-readiness in response to increased frequencies of task switches, reflected in the list-wide proportion switch (LWPS) effect. However, the latent architecture underpinning these adaptations in cognitive stability and flexibility - specifically, whether there is a single, domain-general, or multiple, domain-specific learners - is currently not known. To reveal the underlying structure of control-learning, we had a large sample of participants (N = 950) perform LWPC and LWPS paradigms, and afterwards assessed their explicit awareness of the task manipulations, as well as general cognitive ability and motivation. Structural equation modeling was used to evaluate several preregistered models representing different plausible hypotheses concerning the latent structure of control-learning. Task performance replicated standard LWPC and LWPS effects. Crucially, the model that best fit the data had correlated domain- and context-specific latent factors. Thus, people's ability to adapt their on-task focus and between-task switch-readiness to changing levels of demand was mediated by distinct (though correlated) underlying factors. Model fit remained good when accounting for speed-accuracy trade-offs, variance in individual cognitive ability and self-reported motivation, as well as self-reported explicit awareness of manipulations and the order in which different levels of demand were experienced. Implications of these results for the cognitive architecture of dynamic cognitive control are discussed.

Citing Articles

Principles of cognitive control over task focus and task switching.

Egner T Nat Rev Psychol. 2024; 2(11):702-714.

PMID: 39301103 PMC: 11409542. DOI: 10.1038/s44159-023-00234-4.


Contextual control demands determine whether stability and flexibility trade off against each other.

Geddert R, Egner T Atten Percept Psychophys. 2024; 86(7):2529-2551.

PMID: 39251567 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-024-02955-x.


Insights into control over cognitive flexibility from studies of task-switching.

Egner T, Siqi-Liu A Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2024; 55.

PMID: 38186744 PMC: 10769152. DOI: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2023.101342.

References
1.
Friedman N, Hatoum A, Gustavson D, Corley R, Hewitt J, Young S . Executive Functions and Impulsivity are Genetically Distinct and Independently Predict Psychopathology: Results from Two Adult Twin Studies. Clin Psychol Sci. 2021; 8(3):519-538. PMC: 7983400. DOI: 10.1177/2167702619898814. View

2.
Jiang J, Heller K, Egner T . Bayesian modeling of flexible cognitive control. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014; 46 Pt 1:30-43. PMC: 4253563. DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.06.001. View

3.
Gratton G, Coles M, Donchin E . Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1992; 121(4):480-506. DOI: 10.1037//0096-3445.121.4.480. View

4.
Keye D, Wilhelm O, Oberauer K, van Ravenzwaaij D . Individual differences in conflict-monitoring: testing means and covariance hypothesis about the Simon and the Eriksen Flanker task. Psychol Res. 2008; 73(6):762-76. DOI: 10.1007/s00426-008-0188-9. View

5.
Dey A, Bugg J . The Timescale of Control: A Meta-Control Property that Generalizes across Tasks but Varies between Types of Control. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2021; 21(3):472-489. DOI: 10.3758/s13415-020-00853-x. View