» Articles » PMID: 19034502

Individual Differences in Conflict-monitoring: Testing Means and Covariance Hypothesis About the Simon and the Eriksen Flanker Task

Overview
Journal Psychol Res
Specialty Psychology
Date 2008 Nov 27
PMID 19034502
Citations 29
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Conflict and context slow-down have been proposed as indicators of a conflict-monitoring system that initiates cognitive control to resolve conflicts in information processing. We investigated individual differences in conflict-monitoring and their associations with working memory (WM) and impulsivity. A total of 150 adults completed a Simon and an Eriksen flanker task, together with measures of WM and impulsivity. On both tasks, responses were slower and less accurate on incompatible than on compatible trials (conflict effect), and the conflict effect was larger when the preceding trial was compatible than when it was incompatible (context effect). Stimulus repetition did not explain the context effect. Individual differences could be attributed to three separable factors for each task: general speeded performance, conflict effect, and context effect. Evidence for across-task generality of these factors was sparse. Associations of these factors with impulsivity were weak at best. WM was correlated with general speed, and also with some but not all factors reflecting conflict-related processes.

Citing Articles

Theoretical Considerations on the Literacy-Metacognition Nexus: Exploring the Linguistic-Cognitive Landscape of Young Multilingual Minds.

Hofer B, Spechtenhauser B Brain Sci. 2024; 14(10).

PMID: 39451993 PMC: 11506620. DOI: 10.3390/brainsci14100979.


Psychometrics of drift-diffusion model parameters derived from the Eriksen flanker task: Reliability and validity in two independent samples.

Rappaport B, Shankman S, Glazer J, Buchanan S, Weinberg A, Letkiewicz A Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2024; 25(2):311-328.

PMID: 39443415 PMC: 11908889. DOI: 10.3758/s13415-024-01222-8.


Linking computational models of two core tasks of cognitive control.

Robinson M, Steyvers M Psychol Rev. 2022; 130(1):71-101.

PMID: 36227284 PMC: 10257386. DOI: 10.1037/rev0000395.


Distinct but correlated latent factors support the regulation of learned conflict-control and task-switching.

Bejjani C, Hoyle R, Egner T Cogn Psychol. 2022; 135:101474.

PMID: 35405421 PMC: 9170285. DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101474.


The visual arrays task: Visual storage capacity or attention control?.

Martin J, Tsukahara J, Draheim C, Shipstead Z, Mashburn C, Vogel E J Exp Psychol Gen. 2021; 150(12):2525-2551.

PMID: 34591545 PMC: 9045334. DOI: 10.1037/xge0001048.


References
1.
Simon J . Reactions toward the source of stimulation. J Exp Psychol. 1969; 81(1):174-6. DOI: 10.1037/h0027448. View

2.
Wuhr P, Ansorge U . Exploring trial-by-trial modulations of the Simon effect. Q J Exp Psychol A. 2005; 58(4):705-31. DOI: 10.1080/02724980443000269. View

3.
Kane M, Hambrick D, Tuholski S, Wilhelm O, Payne T, Engle R . The generality of working memory capacity: a latent-variable approach to verbal and visuospatial memory span and reasoning. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2004; 133(2):189-217. DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.189. View

4.
Stahl J, Gibbons H . Dynamics of response-conflict monitoring and individual differences in response control and behavioral control: an electrophysiological investigation using a stop-signal task. Clin Neurophysiol. 2006; 118(3):581-96. DOI: 10.1016/j.clinph.2006.10.023. View

5.
Kornblum S, Hasbroucq T, Osman A . Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. Psychol Rev. 1990; 97(2):253-70. DOI: 10.1037/0033-295x.97.2.253. View