» Articles » PMID: 35346963

Context-Specificity of Locomotor Learning Is Developed During Childhood

Overview
Journal eNeuro
Specialty Neurology
Date 2022 Mar 29
PMID 35346963
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Humans can perform complex movements with speed and agility in the face of constantly changing task demands. To accomplish this, motor plans are adapted to account for errors in our movements because of changes in our body (e.g., growth or injury) or in the environment (e.g., walking on sand vs ice). It has been suggested that adaptation that occurs in response to changes in the state of our body will generalize across different movement contexts and environments, whereas adaptation that occurs with alterations in the external environment will be context-specific. Here, we asked whether the ability to form generalizable versus context-specific motor memories develops during childhood. We performed a cross-sectional study of context-specific locomotor adaptation in 35 children (3-18 years old) and 7 adults (19-31 years old). Subjects first adapted their gait and learned a new walking pattern on a split-belt treadmill, which has two belts that move each leg at a different speed. Then, subjects walked overground to assess the generalization of the adapted walking pattern across different environments. Our results show that the generalization of treadmill after-effects to overground walking decreases as subjects' age increases, indicating that age and experience are critical factors regulating the specificity of motor learning. Our results suggest that although basic locomotor patterns are established by two years of age, brain networks required for context-specific locomotor learning are still being developed throughout youth.

Citing Articles

Understanding mechanisms of generalization following locomotor adaptation.

Rossi C, Roemmich R, Bastian A NPJ Sci Learn. 2024; 9(1):48.

PMID: 39043679 PMC: 11266392. DOI: 10.1038/s41539-024-00258-2.

References
1.
Chevalier N, Chatham C, Munakata Y . The practice of going helps children to stop: the importance of context monitoring in inhibitory control. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2014; 143(3):959-65. PMC: 4057641. DOI: 10.1037/a0035868. View

2.
Mariscal D, Iturralde P, Torres-Oviedo G . Altering attention to split-belt walking increases the generalization of motor memories across walking contexts. J Neurophysiol. 2020; 123(5):1838-1848. PMC: 8086635. DOI: 10.1152/jn.00509.2019. View

3.
Synofzik M, Thier P, Lindner A . Internalizing agency of self-action: perception of one's own hand movements depends on an adaptable prediction about the sensory action outcome. J Neurophysiol. 2006; 96(3):1592-601. DOI: 10.1152/jn.00104.2006. View

4.
Berniker M, Kording K . Estimating the sources of motor errors for adaptation and generalization. Nat Neurosci. 2008; 11(12):1454-61. PMC: 2707921. DOI: 10.1038/nn.2229. View

5.
Cothros N, Wong J, Gribble P . Visual cues signaling object grasp reduce interference in motor learning. J Neurophysiol. 2009; 102(4):2112-20. DOI: 10.1152/jn.00493.2009. View