» Articles » PMID: 33211523

Psycholinguistic Mechanisms of Classifier Processing in Sign Language

Overview
Specialty Psychology
Date 2020 Nov 19
PMID 33211523
Citations 1
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Nonsigners viewing sign language are sometimes able to guess the meaning of signs by relying on the overt connection between form and meaning, or iconicity (cf. Ortega, Özyürek, & Peeters, 2020; Strickland et al., 2015). One word class in sign languages that appears to be highly iconic is classifiers: verb-like signs that can refer to location change or handling. Classifier use and meaning are governed by linguistic rules, yet in comparison with lexical verb signs, classifiers are highly variable in their morpho-phonology (variety of potential handshapes and motion direction within the sign). These open-class linguistic items in sign languages prompt a question about the mechanisms of their processing: Are they part of a gestural-semiotic system (processed like the gestures of nonsigners), or are they processed as linguistic verbs? To examine the psychological mechanisms of classifier comprehension, we recorded the electroencephalogram (EEG) activity of signers who watched videos of signed sentences with classifiers. We manipulated the sentence word order of the stimuli (subject-object-verb [SOV] vs. object-subject-verb [OSV]), contrasting the two conditions, which, according to different processing hypotheses, should incur increased processing costs for OSV orders. As previously reported for lexical signs, we observed an N400 effect for OSV compared with SOV, reflecting increased cognitive load for linguistic processing. These findings support the hypothesis that classifiers are a linguistic part of speech in sign language, extending the current understanding of processing mechanisms at the interface of linguistic form and meaning. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).

Citing Articles

Low-Frequency Entrainment to Visual Motion Underlies Sign Language Comprehension.

Malaia E, Borneman S, Krebs J, Wilbur R IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng. 2021; 29:2456-2463.

PMID: 34762589 PMC: 8720261. DOI: 10.1109/TNSRE.2021.3127724.

References
1.
Krebs J, Wilbur R, Alday P, Roehm D . The Impact of Transitional Movements and Non-Manual Markings on the Disambiguation of Locally Ambiguous Argument Structures in Austrian Sign Language (ÖGS). Lang Speech. 2018; 62(4):652-680. DOI: 10.1177/0023830918801399. View

2.
Emmorey K, Grabowski T, McCullough S, Ponto L, Hichwa R, Damasio H . The neural correlates of spatial language in English and American Sign Language: a PET study with hearing bilinguals. Neuroimage. 2005; 24(3):832-40. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.10.008. View

3.
Strickland B, Geraci C, Chemla E, Schlenker P, Kelepir M, Pfau R . Event representations constrain the structure of language: Sign language as a window into universally accessible linguistic biases. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2015; 112(19):5968-73. PMC: 4434776. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1423080112. View

4.
Roehm D, Bornkessel-Schlesewsky I, Rosler F, Schlesewsky M . To predict or not to predict: influences of task and strategy on the processing of semantic relations. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007; 19(8):1259-74. DOI: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.8.1259. View

5.
Hickok G, Wilson M, Clark K, Klima E, Kritchevsky M, Bellugi U . Discourse deficits following right hemisphere damage in deaf signers. Brain Lang. 1999; 66(2):233-48. DOI: 10.1006/brln.1998.1995. View