» Articles » PMID: 23792806

Cognitive Constraints on Constituent Order: Evidence from Elicited Pantomime

Overview
Journal Cognition
Publisher Elsevier
Specialty Psychology
Date 2013 Jun 25
PMID 23792806
Citations 19
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

To what extent does human cognition influence the structure of human language? Recent experiments using elicited pantomime suggest that the prevalence of Subject-Object-Verb (SOV) order across the world's languages may arise in part because SOV order is most compatible with how we conceptually represent transitive events (Goldin-Meadow, So, Özyürek, & Mylander, 2008). However, this raises the question as to why non-SOV orders exist. Two recent studies (Meir, Lifshitz, Ilkbasaran, & Padden, 2010; Gibson et al., 2013) suggest that SOV might be suboptimal for describing events in which both the agent and patient are plausible agents (e.g. a woman pushing a boy); we call these "reversible" events. We replicate these findings using elicited pantomime and offer a new interpretation. Meir et al.'s (2010) account is framed largely in terms of constraints on comprehension, while Gibson et al.'s (2013) account involves minimizing the risk of information loss or memory degradation. We offer an alternative hypothesis that is grounded in constraints on production. We consider the implications of these findings for the distribution of constituent order in the world's spoken languages and for the structure of emerging sign languages.

Citing Articles

The Agent Preference in Visual Event Apprehension.

Isasi-Isasmendi A, Andrews C, Flecken M, Laka I, Daum M, Meyer M Open Mind (Camb). 2023; 7:240-282.

PMID: 37416075 PMC: 10320828. DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00083.


The Effects of Iconicity and Conventionalization on Word Order Preferences.

Motamedi Y, Wolters L, Schouwstra M, Kirby S Cogn Sci. 2022; 46(10):e13203.

PMID: 36251421 PMC: 9787421. DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13203.


Revisiting the relation between syntax, action, and left BA44.

Kemmerer D Front Hum Neurosci. 2022; 16:923022.

PMID: 36211129 PMC: 9537576. DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.923022.


Crosslinguistic word order variation reflects evolutionary pressures of dependency and information locality.

Hahn M, Xu Y Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2022; 119(24):e2122604119.

PMID: 35675428 PMC: 9214541. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2122604119.


Investigating Word Order Emergence: Constraints From Cognition and Communication.

Schouwstra M, Naegeli D, Kirby S Front Psychol. 2022; 13:805144.

PMID: 35529568 PMC: 9072621. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.805144.


References
1.
Sandler W, Meir I, Padden C, Aronoff M . The emergence of grammar: systematic structure in a new language. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005; 102(7):2661-5. PMC: 548320. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0405448102. View

2.
Goldin-Meadow S, Mylander C . Gestural communication in deaf children: noneffect of parental input on language development. Science. 1983; 221(4608):372-4. DOI: 10.1126/science.6867713. View

3.
Goldin-Meadow S, So W, Ozyurek A, Mylander C . The natural order of events: how speakers of different languages represent events nonverbally. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008; 105(27):9163-8. PMC: 2453738. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0710060105. View

4.
Meir I, Padden C, Aronoff M, Sandler W . Body as subject(). J Linguist. 2012; 43(3):531-563. PMC: 3467700. DOI: 10.1017/S0022226707004768. View

5.
Goldin-Meadow S, Feldman H . The development of language-like communication without a language model. Science. 1977; 197(4301):401-3. DOI: 10.1126/science.877567. View