» Articles » PMID: 31515771

Computer Mouse Tracking Reveals Motor Signatures in a Cognitive Task of Spatial Language Grounding

Overview
Publisher Springer
Specialties Psychiatry
Psychology
Date 2019 Sep 14
PMID 31515771
Citations 3
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

In a novel computer mouse tracking paradigm, participants read a spatial phrase such as "The blue item to the left of the red one" and then see a scene composed of 12 visual items. The task is to move the mouse cursor to the target item (here, blue), which requires perceptually grounding the spatial phrase. This entails visually identifying the reference item (here, red) and other relevant items through attentional selection. Response trajectories are attracted toward distractors that share the target color but match the spatial relation less well. Trajectories are also attracted toward items that share the reference color. A competing pair of items that match the specified colors but are in the inverse spatial relation increases attraction over-additively compared to individual items. Trajectories are also influenced by the spatial term itself. While the distractor effect resembles deviation toward potential targets in previous studies, the reference effect suggests that the relevance of the reference item for the relational task, not its role as a potential target, was critical. This account is supported by the strengthened effect of a competing pair. We conclude, therefore, that the attraction effects in the mouse trajectories reflect the neural processes that operate on sensorimotor representations to solve the relational task. The paradigm thus provides an experimental window through motor behavior into higher cognitive function and the evolution of activation in modal substrates, a longstanding topic in the area of embodied cognition.

Citing Articles

Neural dynamic foundations of a theory of higher cognition: the case of grounding nested phrases.

Sabinasz D, Richter M, Schoner G Cogn Neurodyn. 2024; 18(2):557-579.

PMID: 38699609 PMC: 11061088. DOI: 10.1007/s11571-023-10007-7.


The biased hand. Mouse-tracking metrics to examine the conflict processing in a race-implicit association test.

Di Palma M, Carioti D, Arcangeli E, Rosazza C, Ambrogini P, Cuppini R PLoS One. 2022; 17(7):e0271748.

PMID: 35895706 PMC: 9328548. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0271748.


Hands-on false memories: a combined study with distributional semantics and mouse-tracking.

Gatti D, Marelli M, Mazzoni G, Vecchi T, Rinaldi L Psychol Res. 2022; 87(4):1129-1142.

PMID: 35849179 PMC: 10192188. DOI: 10.1007/s00426-022-01710-x.

References
1.
Tower-Richardi S, Brunye T, Gagnon S, Mahoney C, Taylor H . Abstract spatial concept priming dynamically influences real-world actions. Front Psychol. 2012; 3:361. PMC: 3459017. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00361. View

2.
Ghez C, Favilla M, Ghilardi M, Gordon J, Bermejo R, Pullman S . Discrete and continuous planning of hand movements and isometric force trajectories. Exp Brain Res. 1997; 115(2):217-33. DOI: 10.1007/pl00005692. View

3.
Anderson S, Matlock T, Spivey M . Grammatical aspect and temporal distance in motion descriptions. Front Psychol. 2013; 4:337. PMC: 3697187. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00337. View

4.
Freeman J, Dale R, Farmer T . Hand in motion reveals mind in motion. Front Psychol. 2011; 2:59. PMC: 3110497. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00059. View

5.
Lipinski J, Schneegans S, Sandamirskaya Y, Spencer J, Schoner G . A neurobehavioral model of flexible spatial language behaviors. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2011; 38(6):1490-511. PMC: 3665425. DOI: 10.1037/a0022643. View