» Articles » PMID: 20683612

Learning of Timing Patterns and the Development of Temporal Expectations

Overview
Journal Psychol Res
Specialty Psychology
Date 2010 Aug 5
PMID 20683612
Citations 6
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

The present study investigated the learning of a culturally unfamiliar musical rhythm, leading to the development of temporal expectations, and it explored the potential for generalization across tempi and tasks. With that aim, we adapted the serial reaction time task to examine the learning of temporal structures by an indirect method. The temporal pattern employed was based on a complex interval ratio (2:3) and compared to one based on a simple interval ratio (1:2). In the exposure phase, non-musician participants performed a two-choice speeded discrimination task that required responding by key press to each event of the simple or complex auditory pattern. Participants were not informed about the temporal regularities; their task solely concerned the discrimination task. During exposure (Experiments 1-3), response times decreased over time for both temporal patterns, but particularly for the events following the longer interval of the more complex 2:3 pattern. Exposure further influenced performance in subsequent testing phases, notably the precision of tap timing in a production task (Experiment 2) and temporal expectations in a perception task (Experiment 3). Our findings promote the new paradigm introduced here as a method to investigate the learning of temporal structures.

Citing Articles

Unravelling individual rhythmic abilities using machine learning.

Bella S, Janaqi S, Benoit C, Farrugia N, Begel V, Verga L Sci Rep. 2024; 14(1):1135.

PMID: 38212632 PMC: 10784578. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-024-51257-7.


Task-irrelevant auditory metre shapes visuomotor sequential learning.

Deighton MacIntyre A, Lo H, Cross I, Scott S Psychol Res. 2022; 87(3):872-893.

PMID: 35690927 PMC: 10017598. DOI: 10.1007/s00426-022-01690-y.


Temporal learning in the suprasecond range: insights from cognitive style.

Teghil A, DAntonio F, Di Vita A, Guariglia C, Boccia M Psychol Res. 2022; 87(2):568-582.

PMID: 35344099 PMC: 9928821. DOI: 10.1007/s00426-022-01667-x.


Listeners perceive complex pitch-temporal structure in melodies.

Prince J, Tan S, Schmuckler M Mem Cognit. 2019; 48(4):526-540.

PMID: 31820372 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-019-00987-5.


Thresholds of auditory-motor coupling measured with a simple task in musicians and non-musicians: was the sound simultaneous to the key press?.

van Vugt F, Tillmann B PLoS One. 2014; 9(2):e87176.

PMID: 24498299 PMC: 3911931. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0087176.


References
1.
Povel D . Internal representation of simple temporal patterns. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1981; 7(1):3-18. DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.7.1.3. View

2.
Bremner A, Mareschal D, Destrebecqz A, Cleeremans A . Cognitive control of sequential knowledge in 2-year-olds: evidence from an incidental sequence-learning and -generation task. Psychol Sci. 2007; 18(3):261-6. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01886.x. View

3.
Summers J, HAWKINS S, Mayers H . Imitation and production of interval ratios. Percept Psychophys. 1986; 39(6):437-44. DOI: 10.3758/bf03207072. View

4.
Schmuckler M, Boltz M . Harmonic and rhythmic influences on musical expectancy. Percept Psychophys. 1994; 56(3):313-25. DOI: 10.3758/bf03209765. View

5.
Cleeremans A, McClelland J . Learning the structure of event sequences. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1991; 120(3):235-53. DOI: 10.1037//0096-3445.120.3.235. View