» Articles » PMID: 22936921

Incidental Learning of Temporal Structures Conforming to a Metrical Framework

Overview
Journal Front Psychol
Date 2012 Sep 1
PMID 22936921
Citations 7
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Implicit learning of sequential structures has been investigated mostly for visual, spatial, or motor learning, but rarely for temporal structure learning. The few experiments investigating temporal structure learning have concluded that temporal structures can be learned only when coupled with another structural dimension, such as musical pitch or spatial location. In these studies, the temporal structures were without metrical organization and were dependent upon participants' response times (Response-to-Stimulus Intervals). In our study, two experiments investigated temporal structure learning based on Inter-Onset-Intervals in the presence of an uncorrelated second dimension (ordinal structure) with metrically organized temporal structures. Our task was an adaptation of the classical Serial Reaction Time paradigm, using an implicit task in the auditory domain (syllable identification). Reaction times (RT) revealed that participants learned the temporal structures over the exposure blocks (decrease in RT) without a correlated ordinal dimension. The introduction of a test block with a novel temporal structure slowed RT and exemplified the typical implicit learning profile. Post-test results suggested that participants did not have explicit knowledge of the metrical temporal structures. These findings provide the first evidence of the learning of temporal structure with an uncorrelated ordinal structure, and set a foundation for further investigation of temporal cognition.

Citing Articles

Hierarchical amplitude modulation structures and rhythm patterns: Comparing Western musical genres, song, and nature sounds to Babytalk.

Daikoku T, Goswami U PLoS One. 2022; 17(10):e0275631.

PMID: 36240225 PMC: 9565671. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0275631.


Task-irrelevant auditory metre shapes visuomotor sequential learning.

Deighton MacIntyre A, Lo H, Cross I, Scott S Psychol Res. 2022; 87(3):872-893.

PMID: 35690927 PMC: 10017598. DOI: 10.1007/s00426-022-01690-y.


Temporal learning in the suprasecond range: insights from cognitive style.

Teghil A, DAntonio F, Di Vita A, Guariglia C, Boccia M Psychol Res. 2022; 87(2):568-582.

PMID: 35344099 PMC: 9928821. DOI: 10.1007/s00426-022-01667-x.


Origami folding: Taxing resources necessary for the acquisition of sequential skills.

Zhao F, Gaschler R, Kneschke A, Radler S, Gausmann M, Duttine C PLoS One. 2020; 15(10):e0240226.

PMID: 33017450 PMC: 7535859. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0240226.


Musical Creativity and Depth of Implicit Knowledge: Spectral and Temporal Individualities in Improvisation.

Daikoku T Front Comput Neurosci. 2018; 12:89.

PMID: 30483087 PMC: 6243102. DOI: 10.3389/fncom.2018.00089.


References
1.
Shin J, Ivry R . Concurrent learning of temporal and spatial sequences. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2002; 28(3):445-57. DOI: 10.1037//0278-7393.28.3.445. View

2.
Ullen F, Bengtsson S . Independent processing of the temporal and ordinal structure of movement sequences. J Neurophysiol. 2003; 90(6):3725-35. DOI: 10.1152/jn.00458.2003. View

3.
Tillmann B, Poulin-Charronnat B . Auditory expectations for newly acquired structures. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2010; 63(8):1646-64. DOI: 10.1080/17470210903511228. View

4.
Frensch P, Miner C . Effects of presentation rate and individual differences in short-term memory capacity on an indirect measure of serial learning. Mem Cognit. 1994; 22(1):95-110. DOI: 10.3758/bf03202765. View

5.
Essens P, Povel D . Metrical and nonmetrical representations of temporal patterns. Percept Psychophys. 1985; 37(1):1-7. DOI: 10.3758/bf03207132. View