» Articles » PMID: 19347358

Understanding the Flexibility of Action-perception Coupling

Overview
Journal Psychol Res
Specialty Psychology
Date 2009 Apr 7
PMID 19347358
Citations 10
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

The idea that observing an action triggers an automatic and obligatory activation of an imitative action in the motor system of the observer has recently been questioned by studies examining complementary actions. Instead of a tendency for imitation, cooperative settings may facilitate the execution of dissimilar actions, resulting in a relative disadvantage for imitative actions. The present study aimed at clarifying the contribution of associative learning and interference of task representations to the reversal of congruency effects. To distinguish between the two, an experiment was designed, in which we increased the effects of associative learning and minimized the effects of task interference. Participants completed a series of imitation and complementary action runs, in which they continuously imitated or complemented the actions of a virtual co-actor. Each run was alternated with a test run showing the same actions but including color-cues, and the participants were instructed to respond to color instead of the actor's posture. Reaction times to test runs showed no reversal of facilitation effects between the imitation and complementary action conditions. This result strongly argues that associative learning cannot adequately account for reversed facilitation effects. Our study provides additional support for action-perception models that allow flexible selection of action-perception coupling and challenges the existing models purely based on stimulus-response associations.

Citing Articles

Attention allocation in complementary joint action: How joint goals affect spatial orienting.

Schmitz L, Wahn B, Kruger M Atten Percept Psychophys. 2023; 86(5):1574-1593.

PMID: 37684501 PMC: 11557662. DOI: 10.3758/s13414-023-02779-1.


Social Motor Priming: when offline interference facilitates motor execution.

Betti S, Chinellato E, Guerra S, Castiello U, Sartori L PeerJ. 2019; 7:e7796.

PMID: 31608173 PMC: 6786249. DOI: 10.7717/peerj.7796.


Joint Action: Mental Representations, Shared Information and General Mechanisms for Coordinating with Others.

Vesper C, Abramova E, Butepage J, Ciardo F, Crossey B, Effenberg A Front Psychol. 2017; 7:2039.

PMID: 28101077 PMC: 5209366. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.02039.


Complementary actions.

Sartori L, Betti S Front Psychol. 2015; 6:557.

PMID: 25983717 PMC: 4416362. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00557.


Simulating my own or others action plans?--Motor representations, not visual representations are recalled in motor memory.

Seegelke C, Hughes C, Schack T PLoS One. 2013; 8(12):e84662.

PMID: 24367689 PMC: 3867509. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0084662.


References
1.
Koch I, Allport A . Cue-based preparation and stimulus-based priming of tasks in task switching. Mem Cognit. 2006; 34(2):433-44. DOI: 10.3758/bf03193420. View

2.
Altmann E, Gray W . An integrated model of cognitive control in task switching. Psychol Rev. 2008; 115(3):602-39. DOI: 10.1037/0033-295X.115.3.602. View

3.
Fadiga L, Craighero L, Olivier E . Human motor cortex excitability during the perception of others' action. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2005; 15(2):213-8. DOI: 10.1016/j.conb.2005.03.013. View

4.
Heyes C, Bird G, Johnson H, Haggard P . Experience modulates automatic imitation. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2005; 22(2):233-40. DOI: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.09.009. View

5.
Hommel B, Musseler J, Aschersleben G, Prinz W . The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. Behav Brain Sci. 2002; 24(5):849-78; discussion 878-937. DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x01000103. View