» Articles » PMID: 33484223

"Look! It is Not a Bamoule!": 18- and 24-month-olds Can Use Negative Sentences to Constrain Their Interpretation of Novel Word Meanings

Overview
Journal Dev Sci
Specialty Psychology
Date 2021 Jan 23
PMID 33484223
Citations 3
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Two word-learning experiments were conducted to investigate the understanding of negative sentences in 18- and 24-month-old children. In Experiment 1, after learning that bamoule means "penguin" and pirdaling means "cartwheeling," 18-month-olds (n = 48) increased their looking times when listening to negative sentences rendered false by their visual context ("Look! It is not a bamoule!" while watching a video showing a penguin cartwheeling); however, they did not change their looking behavior when negative sentences were rendered true by their context ("Look! It is not pirdaling!" while watching a penguin spinning). In Experiment 2, 24-month-olds (n = 48) were first exposed to a teaching phase in which they saw a new cartoon character on a television (e.g., a blue monster). Participants in the affirmative condition listened to sentences like "It's a bamoule!" and participants in the negative condition listened to sentences like "It's not a bamoule!." At test, all participants were asked to find the bamoule while viewing two images: the familiar character from the teaching phase versus a novel character (e.g., a red monster). Results showed that participants in the affirmative condition looked more to the familiar character (i.e., they learned the familiar character was a bamoule) than participants in the negative condition. Together, these studies provide the first evidence for the understanding of negative sentences during the second year of life. The ability to understand negative sentences so early might support language acquisition, providing infants with a tool to constrain the space of possibilities for word meanings.

Citing Articles

A Continuity in Logical Development: Domain-General Disjunctive Inference by Toddlers.

Cesana-Arlotti N, Halberda J Open Mind (Camb). 2024; 8:809-825.

PMID: 38974583 PMC: 11226237. DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00148.


The development of using function word "zài" to learn novel verbs in young Mandarin speakers.

Li Z, Liang C, Zhong J, Chen S Psych J. 2023; 13(1):31-43.

PMID: 38105573 PMC: 10917095. DOI: 10.1002/pchj.710.


Language as a mechanism for reasoning about possibilities.

Grigoroglou M, Ganea P Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2022; 377(1866):20210334.

PMID: 36314149 PMC: 9620752. DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2021.0334.

References
1.
Graf Estes K, Hurley K . Infant-directed prosody helps infants map sounds to meanings. Infancy. 2013; 18(5). PMC: 3828035. DOI: 10.1111/infa.12006. View

2.
Waxman S . Specifying the scope of 13-month-olds' expectations for novel words. Cognition. 1999; 70(3):B35-50. DOI: 10.1016/s0010-0277(99)00017-7. View

3.
Austin K, Theakston A, Lieven E, Tomasello M . Young children's understanding of denial. Dev Psychol. 2014; 50(8):2061-70. DOI: 10.1037/a0037179. View

4.
Oshima-Takane Y, Ariyama J, Kobayashi T, Katerelos M, Poulin-Dubois D . Early verb learning in 20-month-old Japanese-speaking children. J Child Lang. 2010; 38(3):455-84. DOI: 10.1017/S0305000910000127. View

5.
Choi S . The semantic development of negation: a cross-linguistic longitudinal study. J Child Lang. 1988; 15(3):517-31. DOI: 10.1017/s030500090001254x. View