» Articles » PMID: 16812445

The Generalized Matching Law As a Description of Multiple-schedule Responding

Overview
Date 1986 Jan 1
PMID 16812445
Citations 15
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

The literature was examined to determine how well the generalized matching law (Baum, 1974) describes multiple-schedule responding. In general, it describes the data well, accounting for a median of 91% of the variance. The median size of the undermatching parameter was 0.46; the median bias parameter was 1.00. The size of the undermatching parameter, and the proportion of the variance accounted for by the equation, varied inversely with the number of schedules conducted, with the number of sessions conducted per schedule, and with the time within a component. The undermatching parameter also varied with the operanda used to produce reinforcers and with the reinforcer used. The undermatching parameter did not vary consistently with component duration or with several other variables. Bias was greater when fewer rather than more schedules were conducted, when two rather than one operanda were used, and when White Carneaux rather than homing pigeons served as subjects. These results imply that the generalized matching law may describe both concurrent and multiple-schedule responding, but that the same variables do not always influence the bias and undermatching parameters in the same way for the two types of schedules.

Citing Articles

Relative reinforcer rates determine pigeons' attention allocation when separately trained stimuli are presented together.

Gomes-Ng S, Elliffe D, Cowie S Learn Behav. 2019; 47(3):245-257.

PMID: 31011893 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-019-00381-w.


Getting It Wrong: Comment on Moore's "Behaviorism and the Stages of Scientific Activity".

Baum W Behav Anal. 2012; 33(2):235-6.

PMID: 22532719 PMC: 2995519. DOI: 10.1007/BF03392223.


Some remarks on the quantitative analysis of behavior.

Marr M Behav Anal. 2012; 12(2):143-51.

PMID: 22478028 PMC: 2742101. DOI: 10.1007/BF03392491.


Response strength in extreme multiple schedules.

McLean A, Grace R, Nevin J J Exp Anal Behav. 2012; 97(1):51-70.

PMID: 22287804 PMC: 3266739. DOI: 10.1901/jeab.2012.97-51.


Closed-economy multiple-schedule performance: Effects of deprivation and session duration.

Elliffe D, Davison M J Exp Anal Behav. 1996; 65(1):111-28.

PMID: 16812778 PMC: 1350066. DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1996.65-111.


References
1.
Reynolds G . Some limitations on behavioral contrast and induction during successive discrimination. J Exp Anal Behav. 1963; 6:131-9. PMC: 1404215. DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1963.6-131. View

2.
FLESHLER M, Hoffman H . A progression for generating variable-interval schedules. J Exp Anal Behav. 1962; 5:529-30. PMC: 1404199. DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1962.5-529. View

3.
Todorov J, de Oliveira Castro J, Hanna E, Bittencourt De Sa M, Barreto M . Choice, experience, and the generalized matching law. J Exp Anal Behav. 1983; 40(2):99-111. PMC: 1347900. DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1983.40-99. View

4.
Baum W . Matching, undermatching, and overmatching in studies of choice. J Exp Anal Behav. 1979; 32(2):269-81. PMC: 1332902. DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1979.32-269. View

5.
Lander D, Irwin R . Multiple schedules: effects of the distribution of reinforcements between component on the distribution of responses between conponents. J Exp Anal Behav. 1968; 11(5):517-24. PMC: 1338520. DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1968.11-517. View