Lapidow E, Bonawitz E
Open Mind (Camb). 2023; 7:855-878.
PMID: 37946850
PMC: 10631797.
DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00110.
Ainslie G
Psychol Addict Behav. 2022; 37(1):13-24.
PMID: 35758981
PMC: 9792619.
DOI: 10.1037/adb0000853.
Taylor H, Smith A, Yi R
Psychol Addict Behav. 2022; 37(1):166-176.
PMID: 35737549
PMC: 10243310.
DOI: 10.1037/adb0000863.
Lefner M, Magnon A, Gutierrez J, Lopez M, Wanat M
J Neurosci. 2021; 41(35):7461-7478.
PMID: 34315810
PMC: 8412996.
DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0438-21.2021.
Canchy L, Girardeau P, Durand A, Vouillac-Mendoza C, Ahmed S
Neuropsychopharmacology. 2020; 46(2):288-296.
PMID: 32731253
PMC: 7853096.
DOI: 10.1038/s41386-020-0786-9.
Episodic curiosity for avoiding asteroids: Per-trial information gain for choice outcomes drive information seeking.
Holm L, Wadenholt G, Schrater P
Sci Rep. 2019; 9(1):11265.
PMID: 31375718
PMC: 6677824.
DOI: 10.1038/s41598-019-47671-x.
Delay of reinforcement versus rate of reinforcement in Pavlovian conditioning.
Austen J, Sanderson D
J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn. 2019; 45(2):203-221.
PMID: 30843717
PMC: 6448483.
DOI: 10.1037/xan0000199.
Time-Preference Tests Fail to Predict Behavior Related to Self-control.
Arfer K, Luhmann C
Front Psychol. 2017; 8:150.
PMID: 28232810
PMC: 5298954.
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00150.
Finding minimal action sequences with a simple evaluation of actions.
Shah A, Gurney K
Front Comput Neurosci. 2014; 8:151.
PMID: 25506326
PMC: 4247113.
DOI: 10.3389/fncom.2014.00151.
Preference on cash-choice task predicts externalizing outcomes in 17-year-olds.
Sparks J, Isen J, Iacono W
Behav Genet. 2014; 44(2):102-12.
PMID: 24442381
PMC: 4032562.
DOI: 10.1007/s10519-013-9638-2.
Rats show preference for delayed rewards on the radial maze.
Feeney M, Roberts W
Learn Behav. 2008; 36(1):42-54.
PMID: 18318425
DOI: 10.3758/lb.36.1.42.
The representation of economic value in the orbitofrontal cortex is invariant for changes of menu.
Padoa-Schioppa C, Assad J
Nat Neurosci. 2007; 11(1):95-102.
PMID: 18066060
PMC: 2646102.
DOI: 10.1038/nn2020.
Self-control in male and female rats.
Van Haaren F, van Hest A, Van de Poll N
J Exp Anal Behav. 1988; 49(2):201-11.
PMID: 16812536
PMC: 1338807.
DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1988.49-201.
Handling time and choice in pigeons.
Shettleworth S
J Exp Anal Behav. 1985; 44(2):139-55.
PMID: 16812429
PMC: 1348173.
DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1985.44-139.
Effects on preference of reinforcement delay, number of reinforcers, and terminal-link duration.
Poniewaz W
J Exp Anal Behav. 1984; 42(2):255-66.
PMID: 16812388
PMC: 1348081.
DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1984.42-255.
Preference in pigeons given a choice between sequences of fixed-ratio schedules: Effects of ratio values and duration of food delivery.
Hall-Johnson E, Poling A
J Exp Anal Behav. 1984; 42(1):127-35.
PMID: 16812378
PMC: 1348049.
DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1984.42-127.
Delay and amount of reward in a concurrent chain.
Snyderman M
J Exp Anal Behav. 1983; 39(3):437-47.
PMID: 16812328
PMC: 1347854.
DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1983.39-437.
Choice behavior of rats in a concurrent-chains schedule: Amount and delay of reinforcement.
Ito M, Asaki K
J Exp Anal Behav. 1982; 37(3):383-92.
PMID: 16812274
PMC: 1333154.
DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1982.37-383.
Choice between rewards differing in amount and delay: Toward a choice model of self control.
Green L, Snyderman M
J Exp Anal Behav. 1980; 34(2):135-47.
PMID: 16812184
PMC: 1332992.
DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1980.34-135.
Titration of schedule parameters by pigeons.
Lea S
J Exp Anal Behav. 1976; 25(1):43-54.
PMID: 16811894
PMC: 1333413.
DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1976.25-43.