» Articles » PMID: 39183856

Motor Competence is Related to Acquisition of Error-based but Not Reinforcement Learning in Children Ages 6 to 12

Overview
Journal Heliyon
Specialty Social Sciences
Date 2024 Aug 26
PMID 39183856
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Background: An essential component of childhood development is increasing motor competence. Poor motor learning is often thought to underlie impaired motor competence, but this link is unclear in previous studies.

Aims: Our aim was to test the relationship between motor competence and motor learning in the acquisition phase. Both reinforcement learning (RL) and error-based learning (EBL) were tested. We hypothesized that slower RL and slower EBL acquisition rates would relate to lower motor competence.

Methods And Procedures: Eighty-six participants ages 6-12 performed a target throwing task under RL and EBL conditions. The Movement Assessment Battery for Children - 2nd edition (MABC-2) provided a measure of motor competence. We assessed EBL and RL acquisition rates, baseline variability, and baseline bias from the throwing task.

Outcomes And Results: In a multiple linear regression model, baseline variability (β = -0.49, p = <0.001) and the EBL acquisition rate (β = -0.24, p = 0.018) significantly explained the MABC-2 score. Participants with higher baseline variability and slower EBL acquisition had lower motor competence scores. The RL acquisition rate was independent of MABC-2 score suggesting that RL may be less of a contributor to poor motor competence.

Conclusions And Implications: Children with slower EBL acquisition had lower motor competence scores but RL acquisition was unrelated to the level of motor competence. Emphasizing the unrelated reinforcement mechanisms over error-based mechanisms during motor skill interventions may help children with poor motor competence better acquire new motor skills.

Citing Articles

A Circle-Drawing Task for Studying Reward-Based Motor Learning in Children and Adults.

van Mastrigt N, Smeets J, van Leeuwen A, van Wijk B, van der Kooij K Behav Sci (Basel). 2024; 14(11).

PMID: 39594355 PMC: 11591306. DOI: 10.3390/bs14111055.


Cross-sectional survey of gender differences in gross motor skills among preschool children in Jinhua City, China.

Chen B, Liu Y, Tang J, Wang J, Hong F, Ye W Heliyon. 2024; 10(21):e39872.

PMID: 39568848 PMC: 11577202. DOI: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2024.e39872.

References
1.
Ingram J, Wolpert D . Naturalistic approaches to sensorimotor control. Prog Brain Res. 2011; 191:3-29. DOI: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53752-2.00016-3. View

2.
Salaj S, Masnjak M . Correlation of Motor Competence and Social-Emotional Wellbeing in Preschool Children. Front Psychol. 2022; 13:846520. PMC: 9019125. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.846520. View

3.
Cabral M, Liu S, Soares N . Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: diagnostic criteria, epidemiology, risk factors and evaluation in youth. Transl Pediatr. 2020; 9(Suppl 1):S104-S113. PMC: 7082246. DOI: 10.21037/tp.2019.09.08. View

4.
Hancock G, Butler M, Fischman M . On the Problem of Two-Dimensional Error Scores: Measures and Analyses of Accuracy, Bias, and Consistency. J Mot Behav. 1995; 27(3):241-250. DOI: 10.1080/00222895.1995.9941714. View

5.
Blank R, Barnett A, Cairney J, Green D, Kirby A, Polatajko H . International clinical practice recommendations on the definition, diagnosis, assessment, intervention, and psychosocial aspects of developmental coordination disorder. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2019; 61(3):242-285. PMC: 6850610. DOI: 10.1111/dmcn.14132. View