» Articles » PMID: 38779689

Knowing That You Know That You Know? An Extreme-confidence Heuristic Can Lead to Above-chance Discrimination of Metacognitive Performance

Overview
Date 2024 May 23
PMID 38779689
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

In daily life, we can not only estimate confidence in our inferences ('I'm sure I failed that exam'), but can also estimate whether those feelings of confidence are good predictors of decision accuracy ('I feel sure I failed, but my feeling is probably wrong; I probably passed'). In the lab, by using simple perceptual tasks and collecting trial-by-trial confidence ratings visual metacognition research has repeatedly shown that participants can successfully predict the accuracy of their perceptual choices. Can participants also successfully evaluate 'confidence in confidence' in these tasks? This is the question addressed in this study. Participants performed a simple, two-interval forced choice numerosity task framed as an exam. Confidence judgements were collected in the form of a 'predicted exam grade'. Finally, we collected 'meta-metacognitive' reports in a two-interval forced-choice design: trials were presented in pairs, and participants had to select that in which they thought their confidence (predicted grade) best matched their accuracy (actual grade), effectively minimizing their quadratic scoring rule (QSR) score. Participants successfully selected trials on which their metacognition was better when metacognitive performance was quantified using area under the type 2 ROC (AUROC2) but not when using the 'gold-standard' measure m-ratio. However, further analyses suggested that participants selected trials on which AUROC2 is lower in part via an extreme-confidence heuristic, rather than through explicit evaluation of metacognitive inferences: when restricting analyses to trials on which participants gave the same confidence rating AUROC2 no longer differed as a function of selection, and likewise when we excluded trials on which extreme confidence ratings were given. Together, our results show that participants are able to make effective metacognitive discriminations on their visual confidence ratings, but that explicit 'meta-metacognitive' processes may not be required.

Citing Articles

Common computations for metacognition and meta-metacognition.

Zheng Y, Recht S, Rahnev D Neurosci Conscious. 2023; 2023(1):niad023.

PMID: 38046654 PMC: 10693288. DOI: 10.1093/nc/niad023.

References
1.
Sherman M, Seth A, Barrett A, Kanai R . Prior expectations facilitate metacognition for perceptual decision. Conscious Cogn. 2015; 35:53-65. DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2015.04.015. View

2.
Stern E, Gonzalez R, Welsh R, Taylor S . Updating beliefs for a decision: neural correlates of uncertainty and underconfidence. J Neurosci. 2010; 30(23):8032-41. PMC: 2896864. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4729-09.2010. View

3.
Rahnev D, Balsdon T, Charles L, de Gardelle V, Denison R, Desender K . Consensus Goals in the Field of Visual Metacognition. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2022; 17(6):1746-1765. PMC: 9633335. DOI: 10.1177/17456916221075615. View

4.
Gigerenzer G, Gaissmaier W . Heuristic decision making. Annu Rev Psychol. 2010; 62:451-82. DOI: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145346. View

5.
Shekhar M, Rahnev D . The nature of metacognitive inefficiency in perceptual decision making. Psychol Rev. 2020; 128(1):45-70. PMC: 7883626. DOI: 10.1037/rev0000249. View