» Articles » PMID: 36604448

Hungarian, Lazy, and Biased: the Role of Analytic Thinking and Partisanship in Fake News Discernment on a Hungarian Representative Sample

Overview
Journal Sci Rep
Specialty Science
Date 2023 Jan 5
PMID 36604448
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

"Why do people believe blatantly inaccurate news headlines? Do we use our reasoning abilities to convince ourselves that statements that align with our ideology are true, or does reasoning allow us to effectively differentiate fake from real regardless of political ideology?" These were the questions of Pennycook and Rand (2019), and they are more than actual three years later in Eastern Europe (especially in Hungary) in the light of the rise of populism, and the ongoing war in Ukraine - with the flood of disinformation that follows. In this study, using a representative Hungarian sample (N = 991) we wanted to answer the same questions-moving one step forward and investigating alternative models. We aimed to extend the original research with the examination of digital literacy and source salience on media truth discernment. Most of the observations of Pennycook and Rand were confirmed: people with higher analytic thinking were better at discerning disinformation. However, the results are in line with the synergistic integrative model as partisanship interacted with cognitive reflection: anti-government voters used their analytic capacities to question both concordant and discordant fake news more than pro-government voters. Furthermore, digital literacy increased detection, but source salience did not matter when perceiving disinformation.

Citing Articles

Spotting false news and doubting true news: a systematic review and meta-analysis of news judgements.

Pfander J, Altay S Nat Hum Behav. 2025; .

PMID: 39984640 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-024-02086-1.


A framework for promoting online prosocial behavior via digital interventions.

Gruning D, Kamin J, Panizza F, Katsaros M, Lorenz-Spreen P Commun Psychol. 2024; 2(1):6.

PMID: 39242897 PMC: 11332127. DOI: 10.1038/s44271-023-00052-7.


Softly empowering a prosocial expert in the family: lasting effects of a counter-misinformation intervention in an informational autocracy.

Orosz G, Farago L, Paskuj B, Rakovics Z, Sam-Mine D, Audemard G Sci Rep. 2024; 14(1):11763.

PMID: 38782940 PMC: 11116454. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-024-61232-x.


Normalized effect size (NES): a novel feature selection model for Urdu fake news classification.

Wasim M, Cheema S, Pires I PeerJ Comput Sci. 2023; 9:e1612.

PMID: 38077597 PMC: 10702998. DOI: 10.7717/peerj-cs.1612.


Evaluation of misinformation among pro-Ukrainian Latvians - the role of prior attitude, analytical thinking, and emotions.

Priedols M, Dimdins G Front Psychol. 2023; 14:1165039.

PMID: 37780159 PMC: 10538560. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1165039.

References
1.
Pennycook G, Rand D . The Psychology of Fake News. Trends Cogn Sci. 2021; 25(5):388-402. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2021.02.007. View

2.
van der Linden S . Misinformation: susceptibility, spread, and interventions to immunize the public. Nat Med. 2022; 28(3):460-467. DOI: 10.1038/s41591-022-01713-6. View

3.
Pennycook G, Rand D . Lazy, not biased: Susceptibility to partisan fake news is better explained by lack of reasoning than by motivated reasoning. Cognition. 2018; 188:39-50. DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.06.011. View

4.
Pehlivanoglu D, Lin T, Deceus F, Heemskerk A, Ebner N, Cahill B . The role of analytical reasoning and source credibility on the evaluation of real and fake full-length news articles. Cogn Res Princ Implic. 2021; 6(1):24. PMC: 8012428. DOI: 10.1186/s41235-021-00292-3. View

5.
Drummond C, Fischhoff B . Individuals with greater science literacy and education have more polarized beliefs on controversial science topics. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2017; 114(36):9587-9592. PMC: 5594657. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1704882114. View