» Articles » PMID: 35143728

Language Control and Code-Switching in Bilingual Children With Developmental Language Disorder

Overview
Date 2022 Feb 10
PMID 35143728
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Purpose: The current study examined language control and code-switching in bilingual children with developmental language disorder (DLD) compared to bilingual peers with typical language development (TLD). In addition, proficiency in each language and cognitive control skills were examined as predictors of children's tendency to engage in cross-speaker and intrasentential code-switching.

Method: The participants were 62 Spanish/English bilingual children, ages 4;0-6;11 (years;months), including 15 children with DLD and 47 children with TLD. In a scripted confederate dialogue task to measure language control, children took turns describing picture scenes with video partners who were monolingual speakers of English or Spanish. The Dimensional Change Card Sort indexed cognitive control, the Bilingual English Spanish Assessment assisted in identifying DLD, and parent ratings from the Inventory to Assess Language Knowledge indexed proficiency in Spanish and English.

Results: Children with DLD were more likely to engage in from Spanish to English (i.e., responding in English when addressed in Spanish) than children with TLD, even when controlling for proficiency in each language. (i.e., integrating both languages within an utterance) did not differ between groups. Cognitive control was more associated with cross-speaker than with intrasentential code-switching.

Conclusions: These findings highlight the need to consider cross-speaker and intrasentential code-switching separately when seeking distinguishing features of code-switching in bilingual children with DLD. The use of increased cross-speaker code-switching by children with DLD especially with Spanish speakers highlights the need for increased support of home language use.

Citing Articles

Code-Switching During Narratives by Bilingual Children With and Without Developmental Language Disorder.

Gross M, Castilla-Earls A Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch. 2023; 54(3):996-1019.

PMID: 37338998 PMC: 10473391. DOI: 10.1044/2023_LSHSS-22-00149.

References
1.
Szekely A, DAmico S, Devescovi A, Federmeier K, Herron D, Iyer G . Timed picture naming: extended norms and validation against previous studies. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput. 2004; 35(4):621-33. DOI: 10.3758/bf03195542. View

2.
Ribot K, Hoff E . "¿Cómo estas?" "I'm good." Conversational code-switching is related to profiles of expressive and receptive proficiency in Spanish-English bilingual toddlers. Int J Behav Dev. 2015; 38(4):333-341. PMC: 4350241. DOI: 10.1177/0165025414533225. View

3.
Snodgrass J, Vanderwart M . A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. J Exp Psychol Hum Learn. 1980; 6(2):174-215. DOI: 10.1037//0278-7393.6.2.174. View

4.
Hartsuiker R, Pickering M, Veltkamp E . Is syntax separate or shared between languages? Cross-linguistic syntactic priming in Spanish-English bilinguals. Psychol Sci. 2004; 15(6):409-14. DOI: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00693.x. View

5.
Crivello C, Kuzyk O, Rodrigues M, Friend M, Zesiger P, Poulin-Dubois D . The effects of bilingual growth on toddlers' executive function. J Exp Child Psychol. 2015; 141:121-32. PMC: 4631405. DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.08.004. View