» Articles » PMID: 34580215

The Cost of Correcting for Error During Sensorimotor Adaptation

Overview
Specialty Science
Date 2021 Sep 28
PMID 34580215
Citations 9
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Learning from error is often a slow process. In machine learning, the learning rate depends on a loss function that specifies a cost for error. Here, we hypothesized that during motor learning, error carries an implicit cost for the brain because the act of correcting for error consumes time and energy. Thus, if this implicit cost could be increased, it may robustly alter how the brain learns from error. To vary the implicit cost of error, we designed a task that combined saccade adaptation with motion discrimination: movement errors resulted in corrective saccades, but those corrections took time away from acquiring information in the discrimination task. We then modulated error cost using coherence of the discrimination task and found that when error cost was large, pupil diameter increased and the brain learned more from error. However, when error cost was small, the pupil constricted and the brain learned less from the same error. Thus, during sensorimotor adaptation, the act of correcting for error carries an implicit cost for the brain. Modulating this cost affects how much the brain learns from error.

Citing Articles

Error fields: personalized robotic movement training that augments one's more likely mistakes.

Aghamohammadi N, Bittmann M, Klamroth-Marganska V, Riener R, Huang F, Patton J Sci Rep. 2025; 15(1):4201.

PMID: 39905053 PMC: 11794892. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-025-87331-x.


Serial dependencies in motor targeting as a function of target appearance.

Tyralla S, Zimmermann E J Vis. 2024; 24(13):6.

PMID: 39641949 PMC: 11629911. DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.13.6.


Attenuation of implicit motor learning with consecutive exposure to visual errors.

Matsuda N, Abe M IBRO Neurosci Rep. 2024; 17:32-37.

PMID: 38910907 PMC: 11190668. DOI: 10.1016/j.ibneur.2024.05.004.


Open Iris - An Open Source Framework for Video-Based Eye-Tracking Research and Development.

Sadeghi R, Ressmeyer R, Yates J, Otero-Millan J bioRxiv. 2024; .

PMID: 38463977 PMC: 10925248. DOI: 10.1101/2024.02.27.582401.


Interaction of dynamic error signals in saccade adaptation.

Wagner I, Schutz A J Neurophysiol. 2023; 129(3):717-732.

PMID: 36791071 PMC: 10027077. DOI: 10.1152/jn.00419.2022.


References
1.
Spampinato D, Satar Z, Rothwell J . Combining reward and M1 transcranial direct current stimulation enhances the retention of newly learnt sensorimotor mappings. Brain Stimul. 2019; 12(5):1205-1212. PMC: 6709642. DOI: 10.1016/j.brs.2019.05.015. View

2.
Yoon T, Jaleel A, Ahmed A, Shadmehr R . Saccade vigor and the subjective economic value of visual stimuli. J Neurophysiol. 2020; 123(6):2161-2172. PMC: 7311721. DOI: 10.1152/jn.00700.2019. View

3.
Kojima Y, Soetedjo R . Selective reward affects the rate of saccade adaptation. Neuroscience. 2017; 355:113-125. PMC: 5510945. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.04.048. View

4.
Marko M, Haith A, Harran M, Shadmehr R . Sensitivity to prediction error in reach adaptation. J Neurophysiol. 2012; 108(6):1752-63. PMC: 3774589. DOI: 10.1152/jn.00177.2012. View

5.
Yoon T, Geary R, Ahmed A, Shadmehr R . Control of movement vigor and decision making during foraging. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2018; 115(44):E10476-E10485. PMC: 6217431. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1812979115. View