» Articles » PMID: 34191637

Effect of Timing Delay Between Visual and Vestibular Stimuli on Heading Perception

Overview
Journal J Neurophysiol
Specialties Neurology
Physiology
Date 2021 Jun 30
PMID 34191637
Citations 1
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Heading direction is perceived based on visual and inertial cues. The current study examined the effect of their relative timing on the ability of offset visual headings to influence inertial perception. Seven healthy human subjects experienced 2 s of translation along a heading of 0°, ±35°, ±70°, ±105°, or ±140°. These inertial headings were paired with 2-s duration visual headings that were presented at relative offsets of 0°, ±30°, ±60°, ±90°, or ±120°. The visual stimuli were also presented at 17 temporal delays ranging from -500 ms (visual lead) to 2,000 ms (visual delay) relative to the inertial stimulus. After each stimulus, subjects reported the direction of the inertial stimulus using a dial. The bias of the inertial heading toward the visual heading was robust at ±250 ms when examined across subjects during this period: 8.0° ± 0.5° with a 30° offset, 12.2° ± 0.5° with a 60° offset, 11.7° ± 0.6° with a 90° offset, and 9.8° ± 0.7° with a 120° offset (mean bias toward visual ± SE). The mean bias was much diminished with temporal misalignments of ±500 ms, and there was no longer any visual influence on the inertial heading when the visual stimulus was delayed by 1,000 ms or more. Although the amount of bias varied between subjects, the effect of delay was similar. The effect of timing on visual-inertial integration on heading perception has not been previously examined. This study finds that visual direction influence inertial heading perception when timing differences are within 250 ms. This suggests visual-inertial stimuli can be integrated over a wider range than reported for visual-auditory integration and may be due to the unique nature of inertial sensation, which can only sense acceleration while the visual system senses position but encodes velocity.

Citing Articles

Visuo-tactile heading perception.

Rosenblum L, Kress A, Schwenk J, Bremmer F J Neurophysiol. 2022; 128(5):1355-1364.

PMID: 36259667 PMC: 9722247. DOI: 10.1152/jn.00231.2022.

References
1.
Townsend B, Legere J, OMalley S, Mohrenschildt M, Shedden J . Attention modulates event-related spectral power in multisensory self-motion perception. Neuroimage. 2019; 191:68-80. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.02.015. View

2.
Stone J, Hunkin N, Porrill J, Wood R, Keeler V, Beanland M . When is now? Perception of simultaneity. Proc Biol Sci. 2002; 268(1462):31-8. PMC: 1087597. DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2000.1326. View

3.
Miller M, OLeary C, Allen P, Crane B . Human Vection Perception Using Inertial Nulling and Certainty Estimation: The Effect of Migraine History. PLoS One. 2015; 10(8):e0135335. PMC: 4539192. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0135335. View

4.
MacNeilage P, Banks M, Berger D, Bulthoff H . A Bayesian model of the disambiguation of gravitoinertial force by visual cues. Exp Brain Res. 2006; 179(2):263-90. DOI: 10.1007/s00221-006-0792-0. View

5.
Butler J, Campos J, Bulthoff H . Optimal visual-vestibular integration under conditions of conflicting intersensory motion profiles. Exp Brain Res. 2014; 233(2):587-97. DOI: 10.1007/s00221-014-4136-1. View