» Articles » PMID: 34160711

Gene × Environment Interactions in the Development of Preschool Effortful Control, and Its Implications for Childhood Externalizing Behavior

Overview
Journal Behav Genet
Date 2021 Jun 23
PMID 34160711
Citations 6
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

This study examined the role of gene × environment interaction (G × E) in the development of effortful control (EC) and externalizing symptoms (EXT). Participants included 361 adopted children, and their Adoptive Parents (APs) and Birth Mothers (BMs), drawn from the Early Growth and Development Study. The primary adoptive caregivers' (AP1) laxness and overreactivity were assessed when children were 27-months-old, and used as indices of environmental influences on EC. Heritable influences on child EC were assessed by the BMs' personality characteristics (emotion dysregulation, agreeableness). Secondary adoptive caregivers (AP2) reported on children's EC at 54 months, and EXT at 7 years. Interactions between BM characteristics and AP1 laxness were related to EC and indirectly predicted EXT via EC. Parental laxness and EC were positively associated if children had high heritable risk for poor EC (BM high emotion dysregulation or low agreeableness), but negatively associated if children had low heritable risk for poor EC (BM low emotion dysregulation or high agreeableness). BM agreeableness also moderated associations between AP1 overreactivity and effortful control, and yielded a similar pattern of results. Our findings suggest that G × E is an important first step in the development of EXT via its effect on EC. Consistent with "goodness of fit" models, heritable tendencies can affect which parenting practices best support EC development.

Citing Articles

Early risk for child externalising symptoms: Examining genetic, prenatal, temperamental and parental influences.

Lee S, Robertson O, Marceau K, Knopik V, Natsuaki M, Shaw D Infant Child Dev. 2024; 33(4).

PMID: 39183803 PMC: 11339911. DOI: 10.1002/icd.2508.


Obstetrical and neonatal complications, prematurity, and childhood effortful control development: A longitudinal twin study.

Pickett J, Ostner S, Murillo A, Clifford S, Doane L, Davis M Dev Psychol. 2024; 60(9):1716-1732.

PMID: 38976431 PMC: 11724834. DOI: 10.1037/dev0001783.


The pernicious role of stress on intergenerational continuity of psychopathology.

Leve L, Oro V, Natsuaki M, Harold G, Neiderhiser J, Ganiban J Dev Psychopathol. 2024; 36(5):2376-2389.

PMID: 38384191 PMC: 11339241. DOI: 10.1017/S0954579424000191.


Parenting styles, empathy and aggressive behavior in preschool children: an examination of mediating mechanisms.

Lin Z, Zhou Z, Zhu L, Wu W Front Psychol. 2023; 14:1243623.

PMID: 38046118 PMC: 10693347. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1243623.


Parenting in the Context of the Child: Genetic and Social Processes.

Reiss D, Ganiban J, Leve L, Neiderhiser J, Shaw D, Natsuaki M Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 2023; 87(1-3):7-188.

PMID: 37070594 PMC: 10329459. DOI: 10.1111/mono.12460.


References
1.
Yamagata S, Takahashi Y, Kijima N, Maekawa H, Ono Y, Ando J . Genetic and environmental etiology of effortful control. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2005; 8(4):300-6. DOI: 10.1375/1832427054936790. View

2.
Moffitt T . Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: a developmental taxonomy. Psychol Rev. 1993; 100(4):674-701. View

3.
Eisenberg N, Spinrad T, Eggum N . Emotion-related self-regulation and its relation to children's maladjustment. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2010; 6:495-525. PMC: 3018741. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131208. View

4.
Bridgett D, Ganiban J, Neiderhiser J, Natsuaki M, Shaw D, Reiss D . Contributions of mothers' and fathers' parenting to children's self-regulation: Evidence from an adoption study. Dev Sci. 2018; 21(6):e12692. PMC: 6202135. DOI: 10.1111/desc.12692. View

5.
Atherton O, Lawson K, Robins R . The development of effortful control from late childhood to young adulthood. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2020; 119(2):417-456. PMC: 7367701. DOI: 10.1037/pspp0000283. View