» Articles » PMID: 31188714

Finding Common Ground in Meta-Analysis "Wars" on Violent Video Games

Overview
Specialty Psychology
Date 2019 Jun 13
PMID 31188714
Citations 21
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Independent meta-analyses on the same topic can sometimes yield seemingly conflicting results. For example, prominent meta-analyses assessing the effects of violent video games on aggressive behavior have reached apparently different conclusions, provoking ongoing debate. We suggest that such conflicts are sometimes partly an artifact of reporting practices for meta-analyses that focus only on the pooled point estimate and its statistical significance. Considering statistics that focus on the distributions of effect sizes and that adequately characterize effect heterogeneity can sometimes indicate reasonable consensus between "warring" meta-analyses. Using novel analyses, we show that this seems to be the case in the video-game literature. Despite seemingly conflicting results for the statistical significance of the pooled estimates in different meta-analyses of video-game studies, all of the meta-analyses do in fact point to the conclusion that, in the vast majority of settings, violent video games do increase aggressive behavior but that these effects are almost always quite small.

Citing Articles

The Game Within the Game: The Potential Influence of Demand Characteristics and Participant Beliefs in Violent Video Game Studies.

Seetahul Y, Greitemeyer T Soc Psychol Personal Sci. 2024; 15(8):943-954.

PMID: 39464782 PMC: 11502265. DOI: 10.1177/19485506241273193.


The impact of acute violent videogame exposure on neurocognitive markers of empathic concern.

Ritchie M, Compton S, Oliver L, Finger E, Neufeld R, Mitchell D Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2024; 19(1).

PMID: 38727544 PMC: 11223611. DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsae031.


Neuroimaging and behavioral evidence that violent video games exert no negative effect on human empathy for pain and emotional reactivity to violence.

Lengersdorff L, Wagner I, Mittmann G, Sastre-Yague D, Luttig A, Olsson A Elife. 2023; 12.

PMID: 37975654 PMC: 10791126. DOI: 10.7554/eLife.84951.


A new variation of modern prejudice: young Korean men's anti-feminism and male-victim ideology.

Jung H Front Psychol. 2023; 14:1230577.

PMID: 37928587 PMC: 10624222. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1230577.


Sensitivity analysis for the interactive effects of internal bias and publication bias in meta-analyses.

Mathur M Res Synth Methods. 2023; 15(1):21-43.

PMID: 37743567 PMC: 11164126. DOI: 10.1002/jrsm.1667.


References
1.
Calvert S, Appelbaum M, Dodge K, Graham S, Hall G, Hamby S . The American Psychological Association Task Force assessment of violent video games: Science in the service of public interest. Am Psychol. 2017; 72(2):126-143. DOI: 10.1037/a0040413. View

2.
Ferguson C, Kilburn J . The public health risks of media violence: a meta-analytic review. J Pediatr. 2009; 154(5):759-63. DOI: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2008.11.033. View

3.
Hilgard J, Engelhardt C, Rouder J . Overstated evidence for short-term effects of violent games on affect and behavior: A reanalysis of Anderson et al. (2010). Psychol Bull. 2017; 143(7):757-774. DOI: 10.1037/bul0000074. View

4.
Kepes S, Bushman B, Anderson C . Violent video game effects remain a societal concern: Reply to Hilgard, Engelhardt, and Rouder (2017). Psychol Bull. 2017; 143(7):775-782. DOI: 10.1037/bul0000112. View

5.
Prescott A, Sargent J, Hull J . Metaanalysis of the relationship between violent video game play and physical aggression over time. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2018; 115(40):9882-9888. PMC: 6176643. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1611617114. View