» Articles » PMID: 30362404

Following Instructions in a Dual-task Paradigm: Evidence for a Temporary Motor Store in Working Memory

Overview
Specialties Psychiatry
Psychology
Date 2018 Oct 27
PMID 30362404
Citations 8
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Evidence from dual-task studies suggests that working memory supports the retention and implementation of verbal instructions. One key finding that is not readily accommodated by existing models of working memory is that participants are consistently more accurate at physically performing rather than verbally repeating a sequence of commands. This action advantage has no obvious source within the multi-component model of working memory and has been proposed to be driven by an as yet undetected limited-capacity store dedicated to the temporary maintenance of spatial, motoric, and temporal features of intended movements. To test this hypothesis, we sought to selectively disrupt the action advantage with concurrent motor suppression. In three dual-task experiments, young adults' immediate memory for sequences of spoken instructions was assessed by both action-based and spoken recall. In addition to classic interference tasks known to tax the phonological loop and central executive, motor suppression tasks designed to impair the encoding and retention of motoric representations were included. These required participants to produce repetitive sequences of either fine motor gestures (Experiment 1, N = 16) or more basic ones (Experiments 2, N = 16, and 3, N = 16). The benefit of action-based recall was reduced following the production of basic gestures but remained intact under all other interference conditions. These results suggest that the mnemonic advantage of enacted recall depends on a cognitive system dedicated to the temporary maintenance of motoric representations of planned action sequences.

Citing Articles

Evidence for separate backward recall and -back working memory factors: a large-scale latent variable analysis.

Byrne E, Gilbert R, Kievit R, Holmes J Memory. 2024; 32(9):1182-1198.

PMID: 39186520 PMC: 11441403. DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2393388.


Hanging on the telephone: Maintaining visuospatial bootstrapping over time in working memory.

Allen R, Havelka J, Morey C, Darling S Mem Cognit. 2023; 52(8):1798-1815.

PMID: 37278958 PMC: 11588821. DOI: 10.3758/s13421-023-01431-5.


The Role of Cognitive Performance and Physical Functions in the Association between Age and Gait Speed: A Mediation Study.

de Maio Nascimento M, Gouveia E, Gouveia B, Marques A, Marconcin P, Franca C Geriatrics (Basel). 2022; 7(4).

PMID: 35893320 PMC: 9326626. DOI: 10.3390/geriatrics7040073.


Translating words into actions in working memory: The role of spatial-motoric coding.

Li G, Allen R, Hitch G, Baddeley A Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2022; 75(10):1959-1975.

PMID: 35084263 PMC: 9424718. DOI: 10.1177/17470218221079848.


Age-related differences in adults' ability to follow spoken instructions.

Jaroslawska A, Bartup G, Forsberg A, Holmes J Memory. 2020; 29(1):117-128.

PMID: 33320055 PMC: 7613123. DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2020.1860228.


References
1.
Garagnani M, Pulvermuller F . From sounds to words: a neurocomputational model of adaptation, inhibition and memory processes in auditory change detection. Neuroimage. 2010; 54(1):170-81. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.08.031. View

2.
Quinn J . Towards a clarification of spatial processing. Q J Exp Psychol A. 1994; 47(2):465-80. DOI: 10.1080/14640749408401120. View

3.
Quinn J, McConnell J . The interval for interference in conscious visual imagery. Memory. 2006; 14(2):241-52. DOI: 10.1080/09658210500210019. View

4.
Smyth M, Pearson N, Pendleton L . Movement and working memory: patterns and positions in space. Q J Exp Psychol A. 1988; 40(3):497-514. DOI: 10.1080/02724988843000041. View

5.
Allen R, Baddeley A, Hitch G . Is the binding of visual features in working memory resource-demanding?. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2006; 135(2):298-313. DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.135.2.298. View