» Articles » PMID: 2266858

Encoding Information for Future Action: Memory for To-be-performed Tasks Versus Memory for To-be-recalled Tasks

Overview
Journal Mem Cognit
Specialty Psychology
Date 1990 Nov 1
PMID 2266858
Citations 26
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

What is the nature of the representation underlying memory for future tasks such as calling the doctor or buying milk? If this representation consists of a verbal instruction that is translated into action at the time of retrieval, then memory should be better when tested via verbatim recall of the instruction than when tested via actual performance. Three experiments rejected this possibility, indicating better memory for a perform mode of report than for a recall mode of report. This was true in Experiment 1 in which subjects saw a series of verbal instructions (e.g., "move the eraser," "lift the cup," "touch the ashtray"), with advance information regarding the mode of report required during testing. In Experiment 2, the advance cue was valid only in 75% of the trials. Memory depended more heavily on the expected mode of report than on the actual mode of report, suggesting that the perform superiority is due to processes that occur during encoding. In Experiment 3, subjects learned 20 phrases depicting minitasks. More tasks were remembered by subjects tested via performance than by subjects tested via verbatim recall. A second part of Experiment 3 also indicated superior memory when a perform test was expected, regardless of which mode of report was actually required. The results were compared with the finding that subject-performed tasks are better remembered than are their verbal instructions, which suggests that the representation underlying memory for future assignments may take advantage of the imaginal-enactive properties of the envisaged acts. Other possible differences between memory for to-be-recalled tasks and memory for to-be-performed tasks are discussed.

Citing Articles

Strategic monitoring improves prospective memory: A meta-analysis.

Peper P, Ball B Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2023; 76(11):2546-2569.

PMID: 37010132 PMC: 10585947. DOI: 10.1177/17470218231161015.


Translating words into actions in working memory: The role of spatial-motoric coding.

Li G, Allen R, Hitch G, Baddeley A Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2022; 75(10):1959-1975.

PMID: 35084263 PMC: 9424718. DOI: 10.1177/17470218221079848.


Age-related differences in adults' ability to follow spoken instructions.

Jaroslawska A, Bartup G, Forsberg A, Holmes J Memory. 2020; 29(1):117-128.

PMID: 33320055 PMC: 7613123. DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2020.1860228.


Following instructions in a dual-task paradigm: Evidence for a temporary motor store in working memory.

Jaroslawska A, Gathercole S, Holmes J Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2018; 71(11):2439-2449.

PMID: 30362404 PMC: 6204648. DOI: 10.1177/1747021817743492.


Forward and backward recall of serial actions: Exploring the temporal dynamics of working memory for instruction.

Yang T, Jia L, Zheng Q, Allen R, Ye Z Mem Cognit. 2018; 47(2):279-291.

PMID: 30284189 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-018-0865-x.


References
1.
Engelkamp J, Zimmer H . Motor programme information as a separable memory unit. Psychol Res. 1984; 46(3):283-99. DOI: 10.1007/BF00308889. View

2.
Cohen R . The effect of encoding variables on the free recall of words and action events. Mem Cognit. 1983; 11(6):575-82. DOI: 10.3758/bf03198282. View

3.
Zimmer H, Engelkamp J . An attempt to distinguish between kinematic and motor memory components. Acta Psychol (Amst). 1985; 58(1):81-106. DOI: 10.1016/0001-6918(85)90036-8. View

4.
Backman L, Nilsson L, Chalom D . New evidence on the nature of the encoding of action events. Mem Cognit. 1986; 14(4):339-46. DOI: 10.3758/bf03202512. View

5.
Marschark M, Richman C, Yuille J, Hunt R . The role of imagery in memory: on shared and distinctive information. Psychol Bull. 1987; 102(1):28-41. View