» Articles » PMID: 27383592

Temporal Dynamics of Sensorimotor Networks in Effort-Based Cost-Benefit Valuation: Early Emergence and Late Net Value Integration

Overview
Journal J Neurosci
Specialty Neurology
Date 2016 Jul 8
PMID 27383592
Citations 13
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Unlabelled: Although physical effort can impose significant costs on decision-making, when and how effort cost information is incorporated into choice remains contested, reflecting a larger debate over the role of sensorimotor networks in specifying behavior. Serial information processing models, in which motor circuits simply implement the output of cognitive systems, hypothesize that effort cost factors into decisions relatively late, via integration with stimulus values into net (combined) value signals in dorsomedial frontal cortex (dmFC). In contrast, ethology-inspired approaches suggest a more active role for the dorsal sensorimotor stream, with effort cost signals emerging rapidly after stimulus onset. Here we investigated the time course of effort cost integration using event-related potentials in hungry human subjects while they made decisions about expending physical effort for appetitive foods. Consistent with the ethological perspective, we found that effort cost was represented from as early as 100-250 ms after stimulus onset, localized to dorsal sensorimotor regions including middle cingulate, somatosensory, and motor/premotor cortices. However, examining the same data time-locked to motor output revealed net value signals combining stimulus value and effort cost approximately -400 ms before response, originating from sensorimotor areas including dmFC, precuneus, and posterior parietal cortex. Granger causal connectivity analysis of the motor effector signal in the time leading to response showed interactions between these sensorimotor regions and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, a structure associated with adjusting behavior-response mappings. These results suggest that rapid activation of sensorimotor regions interacts with cognitive valuation systems, producing a net value signal reflecting both physical effort and reward contingencies.

Significance Statement: Although physical effort imposes a cost on choice, when and how effort cost influences neural correlates of decision-making remains contested. This dispute reflects a larger disagreement between cognitive neuroscience and ethology over the role of sensorimotor systems in behavior: are sensorimotor circuits merely implementing the late-stage output of cognitive systems, or engaged rapidly and interactively from early in decision-making? We find that, although early representation of effort cost is associated with sensorimotor regions, these signals are also integrated with cognitive stimulus value representations in the time leading up to motor response. These data suggest that sensorimotor networks interact dynamically with cognitive systems to guide decision-making, providing a first step toward reconciling differing perspectives on sensorimotor roles in valuation and choice.

Citing Articles

Reward signals in the motor cortex: from biology to neurotechnology.

Derosiere G, Shokur S, Vassiliadis P Nat Commun. 2025; 16(1):1307.

PMID: 39900901 PMC: 11791067. DOI: 10.1038/s41467-024-55016-0.


Neural Determinants of Sedentary Lifestyle in Older Adults: A Brain Network Analysis.

Bahrami M, Burdette J, Laurienti P, Nicklas B, Rejeski W, Fanning J Brain Behav. 2025; 15(1):e70085.

PMID: 39778926 PMC: 11710895. DOI: 10.1002/brb3.70085.


Neurobiological foundations and clinical relevance of effort-based decision-making.

Brassard S, Liu H, Dosanjh J, MacKillop J, Balodis I Brain Imaging Behav. 2024; 18(5):1-30.

PMID: 38819540 DOI: 10.1007/s11682-024-00890-x.


Investigating psychological mechanisms of self-controlled decisions for food and leisure activity.

Bailey C, Lim S J Behav Med. 2024; 47(3):458-470.

PMID: 38342789 DOI: 10.1007/s10865-024-00469-3.


Neurocomputational mechanisms of food and physical activity decision-making in male adolescents.

Lim S, Bruce A, Shook R Sci Rep. 2023; 13(1):6145.

PMID: 37061558 PMC: 10105706. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-023-32823-x.


References
1.
Cavada C, Company T, Tejedor J, Cruz-Rizzolo R, Reinoso-Suarez F . The anatomical connections of the macaque monkey orbitofrontal cortex. A review. Cereb Cortex. 2000; 10(3):220-42. DOI: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.220. View

2.
Jung T, Makeig S, Humphries C, Lee T, McKeown M, Iragui V . Removing electroencephalographic artifacts by blind source separation. Psychophysiology. 2000; 37(2):163-78. View

3.
Jeannerod M . Neural simulation of action: a unifying mechanism for motor cognition. Neuroimage. 2001; 14(1 Pt 2):S103-9. DOI: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0832. View

4.
Creem S, Proffitt D . Defining the cortical visual systems: "what", "where", and "how". Acta Psychol (Amst). 2001; 107(1-3):43-68. DOI: 10.1016/s0001-6918(01)00021-x. View

5.
Paus T . Primate anterior cingulate cortex: where motor control, drive and cognition interface. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001; 2(6):417-24. DOI: 10.1038/35077500. View