» Articles » PMID: 24374020

Independent Effects of Bilingualism and Socioeconomic Status on Language Ability and Executive Functioning

Overview
Journal Cognition
Publisher Elsevier
Specialty Psychology
Date 2013 Dec 31
PMID 24374020
Citations 65
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

One hundred and seventy-five children who were 6-years old were assigned to one of four groups that differed in socioeconomic status (SES; working class or middle class) and language background (monolingual or bilingual). The children completed tests of nonverbal intelligence, language tests assessing receptive vocabulary and attention based on picture naming, and two tests of executive functioning. All children performed equivalently on the basic intelligence tests, but performance on the language and executive functioning tasks was influenced by both SES and bilingualism. Middle-class children outperformed working-class children on all measures, and bilingual children obtained lower scores than monolingual children on language tests but higher scores than monolingual children on the executive functioning tasks. There were no interactions with either group factors or task factors. Thus, each of SES and bilingualism contribute significantly and independently to children's development irrespective of the child's level on the other factor.

Citing Articles

The Effects of Bilingualism on the Executive Control Abilities of the Prader-Willi Syndrome Population.

Garcia-Alcaraz E, Liceras J J Psycholinguist Res. 2025; 54(2):10.

PMID: 40069504 PMC: 11897116. DOI: 10.1007/s10936-024-10123-3.


The association of multilingualism with diverse language families and cognition among adults with and without education in India.

Petrosyan S, Strangmann I, Nichols E, Meijer E, Briceno E, Narayanan S Neuropsychology. 2025; 39(3):223-234.

PMID: 40063370 PMC: 11902890. DOI: 10.1037/neu0000988.


Working Memory as the Focus of the Bilingual Effect in Executive Functions.

Chen J, Li Z, Xiong Z, Liu G Behav Sci (Basel). 2025; 15(2).

PMID: 40001765 PMC: 11851412. DOI: 10.3390/bs15020134.


A longitudinal study of math skills in heritage bilingual children: profiles of strengths and weaknesses.

Bonifacci P, Serena B, Codeluppi F, Peri B Psychol Res. 2024; 89(1):8.

PMID: 39535558 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02045-5.


Individual differences in the perception of phonetic category structure predict speech-in-noise performance.

Myers E, Phillips M, Skoe E J Acoust Soc Am. 2024; 156(3):1707-1719.

PMID: 39269161 PMC: 11401644. DOI: 10.1121/10.0028583.


References
1.
Stevens C, Lauinger B, Neville H . Differences in the neural mechanisms of selective attention in children from different socioeconomic backgrounds: an event-related brain potential study. Dev Sci. 2009; 12(4):634-46. PMC: 2718768. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00807.x. View

2.
Kovacs A, Mehler J . Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009; 106(16):6556-60. PMC: 2672482. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0811323106. View

3.
Morton J, Harper S . What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Dev Sci. 2007; 10(6):719-26. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00623.x. View

4.
Morales J, Calvo A, Bialystok E . Working memory development in monolingual and bilingual children. J Exp Child Psychol. 2012; 114(2):187-202. PMC: 3508395. DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.002. View

5.
Diamond A . Development of the ability to use recall to guide action, as indicated by infants' performance on AB. Child Dev. 1985; 56(4):868-83. View