» Articles » PMID: 23637163

Decay of Motor Memories in the Absence of Error

Overview
Journal J Neurosci
Specialty Neurology
Date 2013 May 3
PMID 23637163
Citations 33
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

When motor commands are accompanied by an unexpected outcome, the resulting error induces changes in subsequent commands. However, when errors are artificially eliminated, changes in motor commands are not sustained but show decay. Why does the adaptation-induced change in motor output decay in the absence of error? A prominent idea is that decay reflects the stability of the memory. We show results that challenge this idea and instead suggest that motor output decays because the brain actively disengages a component of the memory. Humans adapted their reaching movements to a perturbation and were then introduced to a long period of trials in which errors were absent (error-clamp). We found that, in some subjects, motor output did not decay at the onset of the error-clamp block but a few trials later. We manipulated the kinematics of movements in the error-clamp block and found that, as movements became more similar to subjects' natural movements in the perturbation block, the lag to decay onset became longer and eventually reached hundreds of trials. Furthermore, when there was decay in the motor output, the endpoint of decay was not zero but a fraction of the motor memory that was last acquired. Therefore, adaptation to a perturbation installed two distinct kinds of memories: (1) one that was disengaged when the brain detected a change in the task and (2) one that persisted despite it. Motor memories showed little decay in the absence of error if the brain was prevented from detecting a change in task conditions.

Citing Articles

The control of movement gradually transitions from feedback control to feedforward adaptation throughout childhood.

Malone L, Hill N, Tripp H, Zipunnikov V, Wolpert D, Bastian A NPJ Sci Learn. 2025; 10(1):13.

PMID: 40069149 PMC: 11897242. DOI: 10.1038/s41539-025-00304-7.


Audiovisual biofeedback amplifies plantarflexor adaptation during walking among children with cerebral palsy.

Spomer A, Conner B, Schwartz M, Lerner Z, Steele K J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2023; 20(1):164.

PMID: 38062454 PMC: 10704679. DOI: 10.1186/s12984-023-01279-5.


The effects of probabilistic context inference on motor adaptation.

Cuevas Rivera D, Kiebel S PLoS One. 2023; 18(7):e0286749.

PMID: 37399219 PMC: 10317241. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0286749.


Effects of Gradual and Sudden Introduction of Perturbations on Adaptive Responses to Formant-Shift and Formant-Clamp Perturbations.

Chao S, Daliri A J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2023; 66(5):1588-1599.

PMID: 37059081 PMC: 10457088. DOI: 10.1044/2023_JSLHR-21-00435.


The decay and consolidation of effector-independent motor memories.

Bao S, Wang J, Wright D, Buchanan J, Lei Y Sci Rep. 2022; 12(1):3131.

PMID: 35210478 PMC: 8873205. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-022-07032-7.


References
1.
Emken J, Benitez R, Sideris A, Bobrow J, Reinkensmeyer D . Motor adaptation as a greedy optimization of error and effort. J Neurophysiol. 2007; 97(6):3997-4006. DOI: 10.1152/jn.01095.2006. View

2.
Ganesh G, Haruno M, Kawato M, Burdet E . Motor memory and local minimization of error and effort, not global optimization, determine motor behavior. J Neurophysiol. 2010; 104(1):382-90. DOI: 10.1152/jn.01058.2009. View

3.
Shadmehr R, Mussa-Ivaldi F . Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task. J Neurosci. 1994; 14(5 Pt 2):3208-24. PMC: 6577492. View

4.
Shadmehr R . Functional stages in the formation of human long-term motor memory. J Neurosci. 1997; 17(1):409-19. PMC: 6793707. View

5.
Joiner W, Smith M . Long-term retention explained by a model of short-term learning in the adaptive control of reaching. J Neurophysiol. 2008; 100(5):2948-55. PMC: 2585394. DOI: 10.1152/jn.90706.2008. View