» Articles » PMID: 23549660

Motor-sensory Recalibration Modulates Perceived Simultaneity of Cross-modal Events at Different Distances

Overview
Journal Front Psychol
Date 2013 Apr 4
PMID 23549660
Citations 24
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

A popular model for the representation of time in the brain posits the existence of a single, central-clock. In that framework, temporal distortions in perception are explained by contracting or expanding time over a given interval. We here present evidence for an alternative account, one which proposes multiple independent timelines coexisting within the brain and stresses the importance of motor predictions and causal inferences in constructing our temporal representation of the world. Participants judged the simultaneity of a beep and flash coming from a single source at different distances. The beep was always presented at a constant delay after a motor action, while the flash occurred at a variable delay. Independent shifts in the implied timing of the auditory stimulus toward the motor action (but not the visual stimulus) provided evidence against a central-clock model. Additionally, the hypothesis that the time between action and delayed effect is compressed (known as intentional binding) seems unable to explain our results: firstly, because actions and effects can perceptually reverse, and secondly because the recalibration of simultaneity remains even after the participant's intentional actions are no longer present. Contrary to previous reports, we also find that participants are unable to use distance cues to compensate for the relatively slower speed of sound when audio-visual events are presented in depth. When a motor act is used to control the distal event, however, adaptation to the delayed auditory signal occurs and subjective cross-sensory synchrony is maintained. These results support the hypothesis that perceptual timing derives from and is calibrated by our motor interactions with the world.

Citing Articles

Neural correlates of temporal recalibration to delayed auditory feedback of active and passive movements.

Schmitter C, Kufer K, Steinstrater O, Sommer J, Kircher T, Straube B Hum Brain Mapp. 2023; 44(17):6227-6244.

PMID: 37818950 PMC: 10619381. DOI: 10.1002/hbm.26508.


Motor variability modulates calibration of precisely timed movements.

Schlichting N, Fritz C, Zimmermann E iScience. 2023; 26(7):107204.

PMID: 37519900 PMC: 10384242. DOI: 10.1016/j.isci.2023.107204.


The impact of cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) on sensorimotor and inter-sensory temporal recalibration.

Schmitter C, Straube B Front Hum Neurosci. 2022; 16:998843.

PMID: 36111210 PMC: 9468227. DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.998843.


Musical training refines audiovisual integration but does not influence temporal recalibration.

ODonohue M, Lacherez P, Yamamoto N Sci Rep. 2022; 12(1):15292.

PMID: 36097277 PMC: 9468170. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-022-19665-9.


Numerical simulations of one- and two-dimensional stochastic neural field equations with delay.

Sequeira T, Lima P J Comput Neurosci. 2022; 50(3):299-311.

PMID: 35618864 DOI: 10.1007/s10827-022-00816-w.


References
1.
Redding G, Rader S, Lucas D . Cognitive Load and Prism Adaptation. J Mot Behav. 1992; 24(3):238-246. DOI: 10.1080/00222895.1992.9941619. View

2.
Sugano Y, Keetels M, Vroomen J . Adaptation to motor-visual and motor-auditory temporal lags transfer across modalities. Exp Brain Res. 2009; 201(3):393-9. PMC: 2832876. DOI: 10.1007/s00221-009-2047-3. View

3.
Ahrens M, Sahani M . Observers exploit stochastic models of sensory change to help judge the passage of time. Curr Biol. 2011; 21(3):200-6. PMC: 3094759. DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.12.043. View

4.
Griffiths T, Tenenbaum J . Structure and strength in causal induction. Cogn Psychol. 2005; 51(4):334-84. DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2005.05.004. View

5.
Ivry R, Schlerf J . Dedicated and intrinsic models of time perception. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008; 12(7):273-80. PMC: 4335014. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.04.002. View