» Articles » PMID: 20837526

Fusion of Visual Cues is Not Mandatory in Children

Overview
Specialty Science
Date 2010 Sep 15
PMID 20837526
Citations 56
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Human adults can go beyond the limits of individual sensory systems' resolutions by integrating multiple estimates (e.g., vision and touch) to reduce uncertainty. Little is known about how this ability develops. Although some multisensory abilities are present from early infancy, it is not until age ≥8 y that children use multiple modalities to reduce sensory uncertainty. Here we show that uncertainty reduction by sensory integration does not emerge until 12 y even within the single modality of vision, in judgments of surface slant based on stereoscopic and texture information. However, adults' integration of sensory information comes at a cost of losing access to the individual estimates that feed into the integrated percept ("sensory fusion"). By contrast, 6-y-olds do not experience fusion, but are able to keep stereo and texture information separate. This ability enables them to outperform adults when discriminating stimuli in which these information sources conflict. Further, unlike adults, 6-y-olds show speed gains consistent with following the fastest-available single cue. Therefore, whereas the mature visual system is optimized for reducing sensory uncertainty, the developing visual system may be optimized for speed and for detecting sensory conflicts. Such conflicts could provide the error signals needed to learn the relationships between sensory information sources and to recalibrate them while the body is growing.

Citing Articles

Binaural Fusion Sharpens on a Scale of Octaves During Pre-adolescence in Children with Normal Hearing, Hearing Aids, and Bimodal Cochlear Implants, but not Bilateral Cochlear Implants.

Reiss L, Johnson A, Eddolls M, Hartling C, Fowler J, Stark G J Assoc Res Otolaryngol. 2025; 26(1):93-109.

PMID: 39915430 PMC: 11861472. DOI: 10.1007/s10162-025-00975-4.


Feeling safe: a critical look at the effect of neighborhood safety features and perceptions on childhood symptoms of depression.

Infantino E, Barnett T, Cote-Lussier C, Van Hulst A, Henderson M, Mathieu M BMC Pediatr. 2024; 24(1):774.

PMID: 39604905 PMC: 11600559. DOI: 10.1186/s12887-024-05236-6.


Binaural fusion: Complexities in definition and measurement.

Reiss L, Goupell M J Acoust Soc Am. 2024; 156(4):2395-2408.

PMID: 39392352 PMC: 11470809. DOI: 10.1121/10.0030476.


Stereoscopic depth perception through foliage.

Kerschner R, Nathan R, Mantiuk R, Bimber O Sci Rep. 2024; 14(1):23056.

PMID: 39367044 PMC: 11452632. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-024-74666-0.


Multisensory Integration and Causal Inference in Typical and Atypical Populations.

Jones S, Noppeney U Adv Exp Med Biol. 2024; 1437:59-76.

PMID: 38270853 DOI: 10.1007/978-981-99-7611-9_4.


References
1.
Bahrick L, Lickliter R . Infants' perception of rhythm and tempo in unimodal and multimodal stimulation: a developmental test of the intersensory redundancy hypothesis. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2004; 4(2):137-47. PMC: 2738586. DOI: 10.3758/cabn.4.2.137. View

2.
Blake A, Bulthoff H, Sheinberg D . Shape from texture: ideal observers and human psychophysics. Vision Res. 1993; 33(12):1723-37. DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90037-w. View

3.
RAAB D . Statistical facilitation of simple reaction times. Trans N Y Acad Sci. 1962; 24:574-90. DOI: 10.1111/j.2164-0947.1962.tb01433.x. View

4.
Knill D . Discrimination of planar surface slant from texture: human and ideal observers compared. Vision Res. 1998; 38(11):1683-711. DOI: 10.1016/s0042-6989(97)00325-8. View

5.
Kuhl P, Meltzoff A . The bimodal perception of speech in infancy. Science. 1982; 218(4577):1138-41. DOI: 10.1126/science.7146899. View