» Articles » PMID: 19936035

Agree or Agree to Disagree? Assessing the Convergence Between Parents and Observers on Infant Temperament

Overview
Date 2009 Nov 26
PMID 19936035
Citations 47
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

The assessment of infant temperament has been typically accomplished with parent questionnaires. When compared with temperament behaviours observed in the laboratory, parents and observers generally do not agree, leading some researchers to question the validity of parent report. This paper reports on a representative sample of infants whose families resided in non-metropolitan counties and whose temperament was measured in three ways: (1) standard parent report (Infant Behavior Questionnaire); (2) observer ratings across two lengthy home visits; and (3) observer coding of second-by-second reactions to specific emotion-eliciting tasks. In order to account for both trait and method variance, structural equation modelling was applied to a sample of 955 infants (M age = 7.3 months) using variables from the three methods that reflected the dimensions of positivity and negativity. Although models based solely on method factors and trait factors fit the data well, results indicated that a model that included method and trait factors provided the best fit. Results also indicated that parents and observers (either across the home visit or to specific tasks) converge, to a degree, on ratings of the positivity dimension but diverge on the negativity dimension.

Citing Articles

Higher prenatal dietary glycemic index in the third trimester of pregnancy is associated with infant negative affect at 6 months.

Wood E, Nomura O, Ablow J, Elliot A, Horgan A, Nigg J Sci Rep. 2025; 15(1):8357.

PMID: 40069302 PMC: 11897411. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-025-91886-0.


Maternal Emotional Availability Supports Child Communicative Development Regardless of Child Temperament-Findings From the FinnBrain Birth Cohort Study.

Ollas-Skogster D, Korja R, Yada A, Mainela-Arnold E, Karlsson H, Bridgett D Infancy. 2025; 30(1):e12649.

PMID: 39853895 PMC: 11758768. DOI: 10.1111/infa.12649.


Prenatal iodine intake and infant temperament in a multiethnic US cohort.

Akinkugbe A, Duffy J, Kannan S, Hartman T, Landero J, Bosquet Enlow M Public Health Nutr. 2024; 27(1):e226.

PMID: 39501816 PMC: 11645109. DOI: 10.1017/S1368980024001575.


Intergenerational transmission of maternal prenatal anxiety to infant fearfulness: the mediating role of mother-infant bonding.

Rousseau S, Katz D, Schussheim A, Frenkel T Arch Womens Ment Health. 2024; 28(1):157-171.

PMID: 38861169 PMC: 11761977. DOI: 10.1007/s00737-024-01475-9.


How to screen for social withdrawal in primary care: An evaluation of the alarm distress baby scale using item response theory.

Egmose I, Smith-Nielsen J, Lange T, Stougaard M, Stuart A, Guedeney A Int J Nurs Stud Adv. 2024; 3:100038.

PMID: 38746716 PMC: 11080292. DOI: 10.1016/j.ijnsa.2021.100038.


References
1.
Aksan N, Kochanska G . Links between systems of inhibition from infancy to preschool years. Child Dev. 2004; 75(5):1477-90. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00752.x. View

2.
Park S, Belsky J, Putnam S, Crnic K . Infant emotionality, parenting, and 3-year inhibition: exploring stability and lawful discontinuity in a male sample. Dev Psychol. 1997; 33(2):218-27. DOI: 10.1037//0012-1649.33.2.218. View

3.
Goldsmith H, Campos J . The structure of temperamental fear and pleasure in infants: a psychometric perspective. Child Dev. 1990; 61(6):1944-64. View

4.
Putnam S, Stifter C . Behavioral approach-inhibition in toddlers: prediction from infancy, positive and negative affective components, and relations with behavior problems. Child Dev. 2005; 76(1):212-26. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00840.x. View

5.
Pelham B . The idiographic nature of human personality: examples of the idiographic self-concept. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1993; 64(4):665-77. DOI: 10.1037//0022-3514.64.4.665. View