» Articles » PMID: 17363359

The Adaptive Value of Sociality in Mammalian Groups

Overview
Specialty Biology
Date 2007 Mar 17
PMID 17363359
Citations 183
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

According to behavioural ecology theory, sociality evolves when the net benefits of close association with conspecifics exceed the costs. The nature and relative magnitude of the benefits and costs of sociality are expected to vary across species and habitats. When sociality is favoured, animals may form groups that range from small pair-bonded units to huge aggregations. The size and composition of social groups have diverse effects on morphology and behaviour, ranging from the extent of sexual dimorphism to brain size, and the structure of social relationships. This general argument implies that sociality has fitness consequences for individuals. However, for most mammalian species, especially long-lived animals like primates, there are sizable gaps in the chain of evidence that links sociality and social bonds to fitness outcomes. These gaps reflect the difficulty of quantifying the cumulative effects of behavioural interactions on fitness and the lack of information about the nature of social relationships among individuals in most taxa. Here, I review what is known about the reproductive consequences of sociality for mammals.

Citing Articles

The power of interspecific sociality: how humans provide social buffering for horses.

Di Lucrezia A, Scandurra A, Lotito D, Iervolino V, DAniello B, Mastellone V Anim Cogn. 2025; 28(1):20.

PMID: 40072705 PMC: 11903558. DOI: 10.1007/s10071-025-01942-5.


Animal social networks are robust to changing association definitions.

Chan A, Dunning J, Beck K, Burke T, Chik H, Dunleavy D Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2025; 79(2):26.

PMID: 39927187 PMC: 11802709. DOI: 10.1007/s00265-025-03559-7.


Commentary on the Adaptive Significance of Sociality Around Parturition Events, and Conspecific Support of Parturient Females in Some Social Mammals.

Allen Wild C, Yon L Animals (Basel). 2025; 14(24.

PMID: 39765505 PMC: 11672776. DOI: 10.3390/ani14243601.


Group-level signatures in bonobo sociality.

van Leeuwen E, Staes N, Eens M, Stevens J Evol Hum Sci. 2024; 6:e48.

PMID: 39712879 PMC: 11660378. DOI: 10.1017/ehs.2024.44.


Intrinsic and environmental drivers of pairwise cohesion in wild Canis social groups.

Benson J, Keiter D, Mahoney P, Allen B, Allen L, Alvares F Ecology. 2024; 106(1):e4492.

PMID: 39663892 PMC: 11739853. DOI: 10.1002/ecy.4492.


References
1.
Price E . The benefits of helpers: Effects of group and litter size on infant care in tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Am J Primatol. 2020; 26(3):179-190. DOI: 10.1002/ajp.1350260304. View

2.
Cheney D, Seyfarth R, Smuts B . Social relationships and social cognition in nonhuman primates. Science. 1986; 234(4782):1361-6. DOI: 10.1126/science.3538419. View

3.
Seeman T, McEwen B . Impact of social environment characteristics on neuroendocrine regulation. Psychosom Med. 1996; 58(5):459-71. DOI: 10.1097/00006842-199609000-00008. View

4.
Sapolsky R, Alberts S, Altmann J . Hypercortisolism associated with social subordinance or social isolation among wild baboons. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997; 54(12):1137-43. DOI: 10.1001/archpsyc.1997.01830240097014. View

5.
Palombit , SEYFARTH , Cheney . The adaptive value of 'friendships' to female baboons: experimental and observational evidence. Anim Behav. 1997; 54(3):599-614. DOI: 10.1006/anbe.1996.0457. View