» Articles » PMID: 12083677

Response Rate Viewed As Engagement Bouts: Resistance to Extinction

Overview
Date 2002 Jun 27
PMID 12083677
Citations 22
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Rats obtained food pellets by nose poking a lighted key, the illumination of which alternated every 50 s during a session between blinking and steady, signaling either a relatively rich (60 per hour) or relatively lean (15 per hour) rate of reinforcement. During one training condition, all the reinforcers in the presence of the rich-reinforcement signal were response dependent (i.e., a variable-interval schedule); during another condition only 25% were response dependent (i.e., a variable-time schedule operated concurrently with a variable-interval schedule). An extinction session followed each training block. For both kinds of training schedule, and consistent with prior results, response rate was more resistant to extinction in the presence of the rich-reinforcement signal than in the presence of the lean-reinforcement signal. Analysis of interresponse-time distributions from baseline showed that differential resistance to extinction was not related to baseline differences in the rate of initiating response bouts or in the length of bouts. Also, bout-initiation rate (like response rate) was most resistant to extinction in the presence of the rich-reinforcement signal. These results support the proposal of behavioral momentum theory (e.g., Nevin & Grace, 2000) that resistance to extinction in the presence of a discriminative stimulus is determined more by the stimulus-reinforcer (Pavlovian) than by the stimulus-response-reinforcer (operant) contingency.

Citing Articles

Bouts, Pauses, and Units of Operant Performance: A Primer.

Falligant J, P Hagopian L, Newland M Perspect Behav Sci. 2024; 47(3):643-674.

PMID: 39309239 PMC: 11411050. DOI: 10.1007/s40614-024-00419-z.


Dorsomedial prefrontal cortex activation disrupts Pavlovian incentive motivation.

Halbout B, Hutson C, Wassum K, Ostlund S Front Behav Neurosci. 2022; 16:999320.

PMID: 36311857 PMC: 9608868. DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.999320.


Intermittent access training produces greater motivation for a non-drug reinforcer than long access training.

Beasley M, Gunawan T, Tunstall B, Kearns D Learn Behav. 2022; 50(4):509-523.

PMID: 35132517 PMC: 10237344. DOI: 10.3758/s13420-022-00512-w.


Opposing roles for striatonigral and striatopallidal neurons in dorsolateral striatum in consolidating new instrumental actions.

Smith A, Jonkman S, DiFeliceantonio A, OConnor R, Ghoshal S, Romano M Nat Commun. 2021; 12(1):5121.

PMID: 34433818 PMC: 8387469. DOI: 10.1038/s41467-021-25460-3.


Simulating bout-and-pause patterns with reinforcement learning.

Yamada K, Kanemura A PLoS One. 2020; 15(11):e0242201.

PMID: 33180864 PMC: 7660465. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0242201.


References
1.
Gilbert T . Fundamental dimensional properties of the operant. Psychol Rev. 1958; 65(5):272-82. DOI: 10.1037/h0044071. View

2.
Neuringer A, Kornell N, Olufs M . Stability and variability in extinction. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 2001; 27(1):79-94. View

3.
Nevin J . An integrative model for the study of behavioral momentum. J Exp Anal Behav. 1992; 57(3):301-16. PMC: 1323232. DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1992.57-301. View

4.
Mellon R, Shull R . Resistance to change produced by access to fixed-delay versus variable-delay terminal links. J Exp Anal Behav. 1986; 46(1):79-92. PMC: 1348258. DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1986.46-79. View

5.
FLESHLER M, Hoffman H . A progression for generating variable-interval schedules. J Exp Anal Behav. 1962; 5:529-30. PMC: 1404199. DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1962.5-529. View