» Articles » PMID: 8989414

Motor Adaptation to Coriolis Force Perturbations of Reaching Movements: Endpoint but Not Trajectory Adaptation Transfers to the Nonexposed Arm

Overview
Journal J Neurophysiol
Specialties Neurology
Physiology
Date 1995 Oct 1
PMID 8989414
Citations 84
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

1. Reaching movements made in a rotating room generate Coriolis forces that are directly proportional to the cross product of the room's angular velocity and the arm's linear velocity. Such Coriolis forces are inertial forces not involving mechanical contact with the arm. 2. We measured the trajectories of arm movements made in darkness to a visual target that was extinguished at the onset of each reach. Prerotation subjects pointed with both the right and left arms in alternating sets of eight movements. During rotation at 10 rpm, the subjects reached only with the right arm. Postrotation, the subjects pointed with the left and right arms, starting with the left, in alternating sets of eight movements. 3. The initial perrotary reaching movements of the right arm were highly deviated both in movement path and endpoint relative to the prerotation reaches of the right arm. With additional movements, subjects rapidly regained straight movement paths and accurate endpoints despite the absence of visual or tactile feedback about reaching accuracy. The initial postrotation reaches of the left arm followed straight paths to the wrong endpoint. The initial postrotation reaches of the right arm had paths with mirror image curvature to the initial perrotation reaches of the right arm but went to the correct endpoint. 4. These observations are inconsistent with current equilibrium point models of movement control. Such theories predict accurate reaches under our experimental conditions. Our observations further show independent implementation of movement and posture, as evidenced by transfer of endpoint adaptation to the nonexposed arm without transfer of path adaptation. Endpoint control may occur at a relatively central stage that represents general constraints such as gravitoinertial force background or egocentric direction relative to both arms, and control of path may occur at a more peripheral stage that represents moments of inertia and muscle dynamics unique to each limb. 5. Endpoint and path adaptation occur despite the absence both of mechanical contact cues about the perturbing force and visual or tactile cues about movement accuracy. These findings point to the importance of muscle spindle signals, monitoring of motor commands, and possibly joint and tendon receptors in a detailed trajectory monitoring process. Muscle spindle primary and secondary afferent signals may differentially influence adaptation of movement shape and endpoint, respectively.

Citing Articles

Visually induced involuntary arm, head, and torso movements.

Martin A, Bakshi A, Ventura J, Panic A, Lackner J Exp Brain Res. 2024; 242(11):2557-2572.

PMID: 39292248 PMC: 11527965. DOI: 10.1007/s00221-024-06913-9.


Influence of error-augmentation on the dynamics of visuomotor skill acquisition: insights from proxy-process models.

Parmar P, Patton J J Neurophysiol. 2024; 131(6):1175-1187.

PMID: 38691530 PMC: 11381117. DOI: 10.1152/jn.00051.2024.


The effects of probabilistic context inference on motor adaptation.

Cuevas Rivera D, Kiebel S PLoS One. 2023; 18(7):e0286749.

PMID: 37399219 PMC: 10317241. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0286749.


Distinct adaptation processes underlie multidigit force coordination for dexterous manipulation.

Smith M, Hooks K, Santello M, Fu Q J Neurophysiol. 2023; 129(2):380-391.

PMID: 36629326 PMC: 9902226. DOI: 10.1152/jn.00329.2022.


Visual guidance can help with the use of a robotic exoskeleton during human walking.

Kim M, Jeong H, Kantharaju P, Yoo D, Jacobson M, Shin D Sci Rep. 2022; 12(1):3881.

PMID: 35273244 PMC: 8913727. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-022-07736-w.