» Articles » PMID: 39534476

Axial Rotation Affects the Cognitive Characteristics of Spatial Ability

Overview
Journal Front Psychol
Date 2024 Nov 13
PMID 39534476
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Purpose: To test spatial ability in athletes with different axial rotation experience and analyze their behavioral data to explain the cognitive mechanisms of spatial ability in athletes.

Methods: Experiment 1: A total of 147 athletes were selected for the paper-and-pencil mental rotation test (MRT). The athletes were separated according to three sport types: open high-spatial (OH) sport, closed high-spatial (CH) sport, closed low-spatial (CL) sport. Spatial ability testing with a two-factor mixed experimental design of 3 (sport type) × 2 (stimulus type). Experiment 2: In this study, 47 players were selected for computerized mental rotation test, with a three-factor mixed experimental design of 3 (sport type) × 2 (angle: 45°, 90°) × 3 (rotational axis: left-right axis, up-down axis, and front-back axis). Repeated-measures ANOVA was performed to evaluate the data.

Results: (1) The CH group and OH group outperformed the CL group in the non-embodied task (all s < 0.003) and the CH group was better than the other groups in the embodied and tasks (all s < 0.008). (2) Under 45° rotational conditions, the reaction time (RT) for the left-right (LR) and up-down (UD) axes were shorter than that for the front-back (FB) axis (all s < 0.026). However, under 90° conditions, the RT for FB < LR < UD, with superior accuracy and rotational speed for the FB axis than for the LR and UD axes (all s < 0.034). (3) Male players from the CH and CL groups had shorter RTs than did those from the OH group at both angles (all s < 0.047). For female players, the CH group presented a shorter RT than the OH and CL groups did at 90° (all s < 0.006). (4) No sex difference was found for paper and pencil MRTs, but a male advantage existed only in the CL group for computerized MRTs ( = 0.005).

Conclusion: The motor skills associated with axial rotation could promote mental rotation performance and compensate for sex differences in mental rotation ability.

References
1.
Bauer R, Jost L, Gunther B, Jansen P . Pupillometry as a measure of cognitive load in mental rotation tasks with abstract and embodied figures. Psychol Res. 2021; 86(5):1382-1396. PMC: 9177492. DOI: 10.1007/s00426-021-01568-5. View

2.
Peters M, Manning J, Reimers S . The effects of sex, sexual orientation, and digit ratio (2D:4D) on mental rotation performance. Arch Sex Behav. 2007; 36(2):251-60. DOI: 10.1007/s10508-006-9166-8. View

3.
Fargier P, Champely S, Massarelli R, Ammary L, Hoyek N . Modelling response time in a mental rotation task by gender, physical activity, and task features. Sci Rep. 2022; 12(1):15559. PMC: 9481519. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-022-19054-2. View

4.
Vandenberg S, Kuse A . Mental rotations, a group test of three-dimensional spatial visualization. Percept Mot Skills. 1978; 47(2):599-604. DOI: 10.2466/pms.1978.47.2.599. View

5.
Morawietz C, Muehlbauer T . Effects of Physical Exercise Interventions on Spatial Orientation in Children and Adolescents: A Systematic Scoping Review. Front Sports Act Living. 2021; 3:664640. PMC: 8247469. DOI: 10.3389/fspor.2021.664640. View