» Articles » PMID: 38988610

Unconscious and Conscious Gaze-Triggered Attentional Orienting: Distinguishing Innate and Acquired Components of Social Attention in Children and Adults with Autistic Traits and Autism Spectrum Disorders

Overview
Specialty Biology
Date 2024 Jul 11
PMID 38988610
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Typically developing (TD) individuals can readily orient attention according to others' eye-gaze direction, an ability known as social attention, which involves both innate and acquired components. To distinguish between these two components, we used a critical flicker fusion technique to render gaze cues invisible to participants, thereby largely reducing influences from consciously acquired strategies. Results revealed that both visible and invisible gaze cues could trigger attentional orienting in TD adults (aged 20 to 30 years) and children (aged 6 to 12 years). Intriguingly, only the ability to involuntarily respond to invisible gaze cues was negatively correlated with autistic traits among all TD participants. This ability was substantially impaired in adults with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and in children with high autistic traits. No such association or reduction was observed with visible gaze cues. These findings provide compelling evidence for the functional demarcation of conscious and unconscious gaze-triggered attentional orienting that emerges early in life and develops into adulthood, shedding new light on the differentiation of the innate and acquired aspects of social attention. Moreover, they contribute to a comprehensive understanding of social endophenotypes of ASD.

Citing Articles

What is it like to be a lizard? Directed attention and the flow of sensory experience in lizards and birds.

Irwin L Front Psychol. 2025; 15():1424329.

PMID: 39776975 PMC: 11704812. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1424329.

References
1.
Gur M, Snodderly D . A dissociation between brain activity and perception: chromatically opponent cortical neurons signal chromatic flicker that is not perceived. Vision Res. 1997; 37(4):377-82. DOI: 10.1016/s0042-6989(96)00183-6. View

2.
Nummenmaa L, Hietanen J . Gaze distractors influence saccadic curvature: evidence for the role of the oculomotor system in gaze-cued orienting. Vision Res. 2006; 46(21):3674-80. DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2006.06.004. View

3.
Emery N . The eyes have it: the neuroethology, function and evolution of social gaze. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2000; 24(6):581-604. DOI: 10.1016/s0149-7634(00)00025-7. View

4.
Carraro L, Dalmaso M, Castelli L, Galfano G, Bobbio A, Mantovani G . The appeal of the devil's eye: social evaluation affects social attention. Cogn Process. 2016; 18(1):97-103. DOI: 10.1007/s10339-016-0785-2. View

5.
Tomasello M, Hare B, Lehmann H, Call J . Reliance on head versus eyes in the gaze following of great apes and human infants: the cooperative eye hypothesis. J Hum Evol. 2006; 52(3):314-20. DOI: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2006.10.001. View