» Articles » PMID: 38216839

Active and Passive Waiting in Impulsive Choice: Effects of Fixed-interval and Fixed-time Delays

Overview
Journal Learn Behav
Publisher Springer
Date 2024 Jan 12
PMID 38216839
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Behavioral interventions to improve self-control, preference for a larger-later (LL) reward over a smaller-sooner (SS) reward, involve experience with delayed rewards. Whether they involve timing processes remains controversial. In rats, there have been inconsistent results on whether timing processes may be involved in intervention-induced improvements in self-control. Interventions that improved self-control with corresponding timing improvements used fixed-interval (FI) delays, whereas interventions that failed to find corresponding timing improvements used fixed-time (FT) delays. The FI schedule includes a response contingency (active waiting), whereas the FT schedule delivers reward automatically (passive waiting). The present study compared the effects of FI and FT schedules in interventions and impulsive choice tasks to evaluate effects on self-control and timing behavior. The impulsive choice task evaluated preference for an SS option (one pellet after 10-, 15-, 20-, 25-, and 30-s delays) versus an LL option (two pellets after a 30-s delay). The intervention task included forced-choice SS (one pellet after 10 s) and LL (two pellets after 30 s) sessions under FI or FT schedules. FI schedules produced greater sensitivity to SS delay in the impulsive choice task. Both FI and FT interventions increased LL choices. Following choice testing, temporal bisection and peak interval tasks revealed better timing precision for rats with an FI delay experience. Overall, the FI choice contingency was associated with improved temporal attention and timing precision.

Citing Articles

Using sign tracking to experimentally increase self-control in rats.

Mahmoudi S, Madden G J Exp Anal Behav. 2024; 122(3):270-281.

PMID: 39406694 PMC: 11570336. DOI: 10.1002/jeab.4211.


Abbreviated fixed-interval interventions promote self-control in rats.

Panfil K, Smith T, West L, Haas C, Kirkpatrick K Behav Processes. 2024; 222:105112.

PMID: 39393449 PMC: 11560654. DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105112.

References
1.
Rapport M, Tucker S, DuPaul G, Merlo M, Stoner G . Hyperactivity and frustration: the influence of control over and size of rewards in delaying gratification. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1986; 14(2):191-204. DOI: 10.1007/BF00915440. View

2.
Scholten H, Scheres A, de Water E, Graf U, Granic I, Luijten M . Behavioral trainings and manipulations to reduce delay discounting: A systematic review. Psychon Bull Rev. 2019; 26(6):1803-1849. PMC: 6863952. DOI: 10.3758/s13423-019-01629-2. View

3.
Smith T, Panfil K, Kirkpatrick K . Generalizability of time-based interventions: Effects of choice procedure and smaller-sooner delay. Behav Processes. 2022; 196:104584. PMC: 8897259. DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2022.104584. View

4.
Renda C, Madden G . Impulsive choice and pre-exposure to delays: III. Four-month test-retest outcomes in male wistar rats. Behav Processes. 2016; 126:108-12. PMC: 4826824. DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2016.03.014. View

5.
McGuire J, Kable J . Decision makers calibrate behavioral persistence on the basis of time-interval experience. Cognition. 2012; 124(2):216-26. PMC: 3503451. DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.03.008. View