» Articles » PMID: 33584446

Gambling-Specific Cognitions Are Not Associated With Either Abstract or Probabilistic Reasoning: A Dual Frequentist-Bayesian Analysis of Individuals With and Without Gambling Disorder

Overview
Journal Front Psychol
Date 2021 Feb 15
PMID 33584446
Citations 2
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Background: Distorted gambling-related cognitions are tightly related to gambling problems, and are one of the main targets of treatment for disordered gambling, but their etiology remains uncertain. Although folk wisdom and some theoretical approaches have linked them to lower domain-general reasoning abilities, evidence regarding that relationship remains unconvincing.

Method: In the present cross-sectional study, the relationship between probabilistic/abstract reasoning, as measured by the Berlin Numeracy Test (BNT), and the Matrices Test, respectively, and the five dimensions of the Gambling-Related Cognitions Scale (GRCS), was tested in a sample of 77 patients with gambling disorder and 58 individuals without gambling problems.

Results And Interpretation: Neither BNT nor matrices scores were significantly related to gambling-related cognitions, according to frequentist (MANCOVA/ANCOVA) analyses, performed both considering and disregarding group (patients, non-patients) in the models. Correlation Bayesian analyses (bidirectional BF) largely supported the null hypothesis, i.e., the absence of relationships between the measures of interest. This pattern or results reinforces the idea that distorted cognitions do not originate in a general lack of understanding of probability or low fluid intelligence, but probably result from motivated reasoning.

Citing Articles

Gender Differences in the Propensity to Start Gambling.

Diaz A, Garcia J, Perez L J Gambl Stud. 2023; 39(4):1799-1814.

PMID: 37402116 PMC: 10627930. DOI: 10.1007/s10899-023-10232-z.


Cognitive Factors that Predict Gambling Fallacy Endorsement.

Shaw C, Williams R, Violo V, Williams J, Demetrovics Z, Delfabbro P J Gambl Stud. 2022; 39(2):843-855.

PMID: 36565358 DOI: 10.1007/s10899-022-10177-9.

References
1.
Goodie A, Fortune E . Measuring cognitive distortions in pathological gambling: review and meta-analyses. Psychol Addict Behav. 2013; 27(3):730-43. DOI: 10.1037/a0031892. View

2.
Xue G, Lu Z, Levin I, Bechara A . An fMRI study of risk-taking following wins and losses: implications for the gambler's fallacy. Hum Brain Mapp. 2011; 32(2):271-81. PMC: 3429350. DOI: 10.1002/hbm.21015. View

3.
Del Prete F, Steward T, Navas J, Fernandez-Aranda F, Jimenez-Murcia S, Oei T . The role of affect-driven impulsivity in gambling cognitions: A convenience-sample study with a Spanish version of the Gambling-Related Cognitions Scale. J Behav Addict. 2017; 6(1):51-63. PMC: 5572993. DOI: 10.1556/2006.6.2017.001. View

4.
Barrada J, Navas J, Ruiz de Lara C, Billieux J, Devos G, Perales J . Reconsidering the roots, structure, and implications of gambling motives: An integrative approach. PLoS One. 2019; 14(2):e0212695. PMC: 6386301. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0212695. View

5.
Lesieur H, Blume S . The South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS): a new instrument for the identification of pathological gamblers. Am J Psychiatry. 1987; 144(9):1184-8. DOI: 10.1176/ajp.144.9.1184. View