» Articles » PMID: 33013498

Family Conversations About Heat and Temperature: Implications for Children's Learning

Overview
Journal Front Psychol
Date 2020 Oct 5
PMID 33013498
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Some science educators claim that children enter science classrooms with a conception of heat considered by physicists to be incorrect and speculate that "misconceptions" may result from the way heat is talked about in everyday language (e.g., Lautrey and Mazens, 2004; Slotta and Chi, 2006). We investigated talk about heat in naturalistic conversation to explore the claim that children often hear heat discussed as a substance rather than as a process, potentially hindering later learning of heat as energy involved in emergent processes. We explored naturalistic speech among children and adults to understand the nature and the frequency of heat- and temperature-related conversations that young children are involved in. This study aims to investigate the actual linguistic resources that children have available as part of a sociocultural approach to cognitive development. Parents' everyday conversations about heat and temperature with their 2-6-year-old children were drawn from the Child Language Data Exchange System (CHILDES) language database and from a parent-child book-reading study. Parents used the word rarely, but they did so in ways that implied it is a substance. Parents never talked about heat as an emergent process but sometimes as a direct causal process. Most of the heat- and temperature-related talk, however, focused on words like and to describe temperature as a property of objects. This investigation of what young children actually experience in everyday conversations is a step toward studying how everyday language may play a role in children's understanding of heat and temperature.

References
1.
Legare C, Evans E, Rosengren K, Harris P . The coexistence of natural and supernatural explanations across cultures and development. Child Dev. 2012; 83(3):779-93. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01743.x. View

2.
. Scienceblind: Why Our Intuitive Theories About the World Are So Often Wrong. Science. 2017; 356(6336):385. DOI: 10.1126/science.aan4200. View

3.
Siegler R . Cognitive variability. Dev Sci. 2006; 10(1):104-9. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00571.x. View

4.
Gelman S . Learning from others: children's construction of concepts. Annu Rev Psychol. 2008; 60:115-40. PMC: 2829654. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093659. View

5.
Harris P, Koenig M . Trust in testimony: how children learn about science and religion. Child Dev. 2006; 77(3):505-24. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00886.x. View