» Articles » PMID: 32539487

Cognition Test Battery: Adjusting for Practice and Stimulus Set Effects for Varying Administration Intervals in High Performing Individuals

Overview
Publisher Routledge
Date 2020 Jun 17
PMID 32539487
Citations 10
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Introduction: Practice effects associated with the repeated administration of cognitive tests often confound true therapeutic or experimental effects. Alternate test forms help reduce practice effects, but generating stimulus sets with identical properties can be difficult. The main objective of this study was to disentangle practice and stimulus set effects for , a battery of 10 brief cognitive tests specifically designed for high-performing populations with 15 unique versions for repeated testing. A secondary objective was to investigate the effects of test-retest interval on practice effects.

Methods: The 15 versions of were administered in three groups of 15-16 subjects (total N = 46, mean±SD age 32.5 ± 7.2 years, range 25-54 years, 23 male) in a randomized but balanced fashion with administration intervals of ≥10 days, ≤5 days, or 4 times per day. Mixed effect models were used to investigate linear and logarithmic trends across repeated administrations in key speed and accuracy outcomes, whether these trends differed significantly between administration interval groups, and whether stimulus sets differed significantly in difficulty.

Results: Protracted, non-linear practice effects well beyond the second administration were observed for most of the 10 tests both in accuracy and speed, but test-retest administration interval significantly affected practice effects only for 3 out of the 10 tests and only in the speed domain. Stimulus set effects were observed for the 6 tests that use unique sets of stimuli. Factors were established that allow for correcting for both practice and stimulus set effects.

Conclusions: Practice effects are pronounced and probably under-appreciated in cognitive testing. The correction factors established in this study are a unique feature of the battery that can help avoid masking practice effects, address noise generated by differences in stimulus set difficulty, and facilitate interpretation of results from studies with repeated assessments.

Citing Articles

Cognitive performance in ISS astronauts on 6-month low earth orbit missions.

Dev S, Khader A, Begerowski S, Anderson S, Clement G, Bell S Front Physiol. 2024; 15:1451269.

PMID: 39633651 PMC: 11614644. DOI: 10.3389/fphys.2024.1451269.


Molecular and physiological changes in the SpaceX Inspiration4 civilian crew.

Jones C, Overbey E, Lacombe J, Ecker A, Meydan C, Ryon K Nature. 2024; 632(8027):1155-1164.

PMID: 38862026 PMC: 11357997. DOI: 10.1038/s41586-024-07648-x.


Effects of additive sensory noise on cognition.

Sherman S, Greenstein M, Basner M, Clark T, Anderson A Front Hum Neurosci. 2023; 17:1092154.

PMID: 37333835 PMC: 10270290. DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2023.1092154.


Military tactical adaptive decision making during simulated military operational stress is influenced by personality, resilience, aerobic fitness, and neurocognitive function.

Sekel N, Beckner M, Conkright W, LaGoy A, Proessl F, Lovalekar M Front Psychol. 2023; 14:1102425.

PMID: 36844343 PMC: 9944034. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1102425.


Future research directions to identify risks and mitigation strategies for neurostructural, ocular, and behavioral changes induced by human spaceflight: A NASA-ESA expert group consensus report.

Seidler R, Stern C, Basner M, Stahn A, Wuyts F, Eulenburg P Front Neural Circuits. 2022; 16:876789.

PMID: 35991346 PMC: 9387435. DOI: 10.3389/fncir.2022.876789.


References
1.
Glahn D, Gur R, Ragland J, Censits D, Gur R . Reliability, performance characteristics, construct validity, and an initial clinical application of a visual object learning test (VOLT). Neuropsychology. 1997; 11(4):602-612. DOI: 10.1037//0894-4105.11.4.602. View

2.
Duff K . Evidence-based indicators of neuropsychological change in the individual patient: relevant concepts and methods. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2012; 27(3):248-61. PMC: 3499091. DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acr120. View

3.
Moore T, Reise S, Gur R, Hakonarson H, Gur R . Psychometric properties of the Penn Computerized Neurocognitive Battery. Neuropsychology. 2014; 29(2):235-46. PMC: 4345134. DOI: 10.1037/neu0000093. View

4.
Gur R, Richard J, Calkins M, Chiavacci R, Hansen J, Bilker W . Age group and sex differences in performance on a computerized neurocognitive battery in children age 8-21. Neuropsychology. 2012; 26(2):251-265. PMC: 3295891. DOI: 10.1037/a0026712. View

5.
Heilbronner R, Sweet J, Attix D, Krull K, Henry G, Hart R . Official position of the American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology on serial neuropsychological assessments: the utility and challenges of repeat test administrations in clinical and forensic contexts. Clin Neuropsychol. 2010; 24(8):1267-78. DOI: 10.1080/13854046.2010.526785. View