» Articles » PMID: 32441736

Multiple Object-tracking Isolates Feedback-specific Load in Attention and Learning

Overview
Journal J Vis
Specialty Ophthalmology
Date 2020 May 23
PMID 32441736
Citations 3
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Feedback is beneficial for learning. Nevertheless, it remains unclear whether (i) feedback draws attentional resources when integrated and (ii) the benefits of feedback for learning can be demonstrated using an attention-based task. We therefore (i) isolated feedback-specific load from task-specific load via individual differences in attention resource capacity and (ii) examined the effect of trial-by-trial feedback (i.e., present vs. absent) on learning a multiple object-tracking (MOT) paradigm. We chose MOT because it is a robust measure of attention resource capacity. In Study 1 participants tracked one (i.e., lowest attentional load condition) through four target items (i.e., highest load condition) among eight total items. One group (n = 32) received trial-by-trial feedback whereas the other group (n = 32) did not. The absence of feedback resulted in better MOT performance compared with the presence of feedback. Moreover, the difference in MOT capability between groups increased as the task-specific attentional load increased. These findings suggest that feedback integration requires attentional resources. Study 2 examined whether the absence (n = 19) or presence (n = 19) of feedback affects learning on the same MOT task across four testing days. When holding task-specific load constant, improvement in MOT was greater with feedback than without. Although this study is the first to isolate feedback-specific load in attention with MOT, more evidence is needed to demonstrate how the benefits of feedback translate to improvement on an attention-based task. These findings encourage future research to further explore the interaction between feedback, attention and learning.

Citing Articles

The impact of impulsivity and compulsivity on error processing in different motivational contexts.

Overmeyer R, Endrass T Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2025; .

PMID: 40045099 DOI: 10.3758/s13415-025-01281-5.


Multiple object tracking training affects the executive function in basketball players: the role of instant feedback.

Xiao W, Jiang Z BMC Psychol. 2024; 12(1):417.

PMID: 39085918 PMC: 11293194. DOI: 10.1186/s40359-024-01931-2.


Unraveling the influence of trial-based motivational changes on performance monitoring stages in a flanker task.

Overmeyer R, Kirschner H, Fischer A, Endrass T Sci Rep. 2023; 13(1):19180.

PMID: 37932359 PMC: 10628251. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-023-45526-0.


Characterizing Attention Resource Capacity in Autism: A Multiple Object Tracking Study.

Tullo D, Levy B, Faubert J, Bertone A J Autism Dev Disord. 2023; 54(8):2802-2815.

PMID: 37344729 PMC: 11300490. DOI: 10.1007/s10803-023-05974-z.

References
1.
Meyerhoff H, Papenmeier F, Huff M . Studying visual attention using the multiple object tracking paradigm: A tutorial review. Atten Percept Psychophys. 2017; 79(5):1255-1274. DOI: 10.3758/s13414-017-1338-1. View

2.
Evers K, de-Wit L, Van der Hallen R, Haesen B, Steyaert J, Noens I . Brief report: Reduced grouping interference in children with ASD: evidence from a Multiple Object Tracking Task. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014; 44(7):1779-87. DOI: 10.1007/s10803-013-2031-4. View

3.
Fougnie D, Marois R . Distinct capacity limits for attention and working memory: Evidence from attentive tracking and visual working memory paradigms. Psychol Sci. 2006; 17(6):526-34. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01739.x. View

4.
Trick L, Perl T, Sethi N . Age-related differences in multiple-object tracking. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2005; 60(2):P102-5. DOI: 10.1093/geronb/60.2.p102. View

5.
Maddox W, Ashby F, Ing A, Pickering A . Disrupting feedback processing interferes with rule-based but not information-integration category learning. Mem Cognit. 2004; 32(4):582-91. DOI: 10.3758/bf03195849. View