» Articles » PMID: 31849757

Where Are the Months? Mental Images of Circular Time in a Large Online Sample

Overview
Journal Front Psychol
Date 2019 Dec 19
PMID 31849757
Citations 4
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

People may think about time by mentally imaging it in some spatial form, or as "spacetime." In an online survey, 76,922 Norwegian individuals positioned two dots corresponding to the months of December and March on what they imagined to be their appropriate places on a circle. The majority of respondents placed December within a section of the circumference ranging from 11:00 to 12:00 o'clock, but a group of respondents chose positions around the diametrically opposite 6:00 o'clock position. A similar relationship occurred for March, where most respondents chose a position ranging from 2:30 to 3:00 o'clock but a group of respondents chose positions around 9:00 o'clock. About half of the respondents ( = 39,797) continued to fill out an online questionnaire probing their mental images related to the "year" concept. This clarified that 75% of respondents "saw" the months unfolding in a clockwise direction versus 19% in a counter clockwise fashion. Moreover, while a majority (70%) stated that they imagined the year as a "circle," the rest indicated the use of other mental images (e.g., ellipses and spirals, lines and squares, idiosyncratic or synesthetic spatial forms). We found only weak effects or preferences for spatial forms based on respondents' gender, handedness, age, or geographical location.

Citing Articles

Keeping track of time: Horizontal spatial biases for hours, days, and months.

Malyshevskaya A, Miklashevsky A, Fischer M, Scheepers C, Shtyrov Y, Myachykov A Mem Cognit. 2023; 52(4):894-908.

PMID: 38153647 PMC: 11111500. DOI: 10.3758/s13421-023-01508-1.


A pilot study of how the past, present, and future are represented in three-dimensional space.

Yabe Y, Yamada S Front Psychol. 2023; 14:1071917.

PMID: 37034943 PMC: 10075084. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1071917.


Gearing Time Toward Musical Creativity: Conceptual Integration and Material Anchoring in Xenakis' .

Besada J, Barthel-Calvet A, Pagan Canovas C Front Psychol. 2021; 11:611316.

PMID: 33536978 PMC: 7849451. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.611316.


Delay discounting decisions are linked to temporal distance representations of world events across cultures.

Croote D, Lai B, Hu J, Baxter M, Montagrin A, Schiller D Sci Rep. 2020; 10(1):12913.

PMID: 32737357 PMC: 7395128. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-020-69700-w.

References
1.
Zorzi M, Priftis K, Meneghello F, Marenzi R, Umilta C . The spatial representation of numerical and non-numerical sequences: evidence from neglect. Neuropsychologia. 2005; 44(7):1061-7. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.10.025. View

2.
Hubbard E, Ranzini M, Piazza M, Dehaene S . What information is critical to elicit interference in number-form synaesthesia?. Cortex. 2009; 45(10):1200-16. DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.06.011. View

3.
Laeng B, Sulutvedt U . The eye pupil adjusts to imaginary light. Psychol Sci. 2013; 25(1):188-97. DOI: 10.1177/0956797613503556. View

4.
Wearden J . Passage of time judgements. Conscious Cogn. 2015; 38:165-71. DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2015.06.005. View

5.
Eichenbaum H . On the Integration of Space, Time, and Memory. Neuron. 2017; 95(5):1007-1018. PMC: 5662113. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.036. View