» Articles » PMID: 31112436

Working Memory Profiles of Children With Dyslexia, Developmental Language Disorder, or Both

Overview
Date 2019 May 22
PMID 31112436
Citations 31
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Purpose Compared to children with typical development, children with dyslexia, developmental language disorder (DLD), or both often demonstrate working memory deficits. It is unclear how pervasive the deficits are or whether the deficits align with diagnostic category. The purpose of this study was to determine whether different working memory profiles would emerge on a comprehensive battery of central executive, phonological, visuospatial, and binding working memory tasks and whether these profiles were associated with group membership. Method Three hundred two 2nd graders with typical development, dyslexia, DLD, or dyslexia/DLD completed 13 tasks from the Comprehensive Assessment Battery for Children-Working Memory ( Gray, Alt, Hogan, Green, & Cowan, n.d. ) that assessed central executive, phonological, and visuospatial/attention components of working memory. Results Latent class analyses yielded 4 distinct latent classes: low overall (21%), average with high number updating (30%), average with low number updating (12%), and high overall (37%). Children from each disability group and children from the typically developing group were present in each class. Discussion Findings highlight the importance of knowing an individual child's working memory profile because working memory profiles are not synonymous with learning disabilities diagnosis. Thus, working memory assessments could contribute important information about children's cognitive function over and above typical psychoeducational measures.

Citing Articles

Processing of Scene-Grammar Inconsistencies in Children with Developmental Language Disorder-Insights from Implicit and Explicit Measures.

Bahn D, Turk D, Tsenkova N, Schwarzer G, Vo M, Kauschke C Brain Sci. 2025; 15(2).

PMID: 40002472 PMC: 11852763. DOI: 10.3390/brainsci15020139.


Development and Validity of the Adult Reading History Questionnaire (ARHQ) for Chinese.

He Y, Tang J, Yang X, Song Z, Ding N, Jia Y Dyslexia. 2025; 31(1):e1802.

PMID: 39907021 PMC: 11795345. DOI: 10.1002/dys.1802.


Are there distinct subtypes of developmental dyslexia?.

Chalmpe M, Vlachos F Front Behav Neurosci. 2025; 18():1512892.

PMID: 39830986 PMC: 11739093. DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2024.1512892.


Assessing the relationship between dyslexia, psychological distress, and academic self-efficacy among Nigerian university undergraduates.

Akpunne B, Idowu O, Kumuyi D, Akpunne E J Educ Health Promot. 2024; 13:315.

PMID: 39429819 PMC: 11488781. DOI: 10.4103/jehp.jehp_1652_23.


Response to intervention as an identification strategy of the risk for dyslexia.

Medda M, Barbosa T, Rocco I, Mello C Codas. 2024; 36(4):e20230031.

PMID: 38865500 PMC: 11189365. DOI: 10.1590/2317-1782/20242023031pt.


References
1.
Fung W, Swanson H . Working memory components that predict word problem solving: Is it merely a function of reading, calculation, and fluid intelligence?. Mem Cognit. 2017; 45(5):804-823. DOI: 10.3758/s13421-017-0697-0. View

2.
Archibald L, Joanisse M . On the sensitivity and specificity of nonword repetition and sentence recall to language and memory impairments in children. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2009; 52(4):899-914. DOI: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0099). View

3.
Alt M, Hogan T, Green S, Gray S, Cabbage K, Cowan N . Word Learning Deficits in Children With Dyslexia. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2017; 60(4):1012-1028. PMC: 5548075. DOI: 10.1044/2016_JSLHR-L-16-0036. View

4.
Baddeley . The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory?. Trends Cogn Sci. 2000; 4(11):417-423. DOI: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01538-2. View

5.
Swanson H, Berninger V . Individual differences in children's working memory and writing skill. J Exp Child Psychol. 1996; 63(2):358-85. DOI: 10.1006/jecp.1996.0054. View