» Articles » PMID: 30348077

Language Delay Aggregates in Toddler Siblings of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Overview
Publisher Biomed Central
Specialties Neurology
Psychiatry
Date 2018 Oct 24
PMID 30348077
Citations 29
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Background: Language delay is extremely common in children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), yet it is unclear whether measurable variation in early language is associated with genetic liability for ASD. Assessment of language development in unaffected siblings of children with ASD can inform whether decreased early language ability aggregates with inherited risk for ASD and serves as an ASD endophenotype.

Methods: We implemented two approaches: (1) a meta-analysis of studies comparing language delay, a categorical indicator of language function, and language scores, a continuous metric, in unaffected toddlers at high and low familial risk for ASD, and (2) a parallel analysis of 350 unaffected 24-month-olds in the Infant Brain Imaging Study (IBIS), a prospective study of infants at high and low familial risk for ASD. An advantage of the former was its detection of group differences from pooled data across unique samples; an advantage of the latter was its sensitivity in quantifying early manifestations of language delay while accounting for covariates within a single large sample.

Results: Meta-analysis showed that high-risk siblings without ASD (HR-noASD) were three to four times more likely to exhibit language delay versus low-risk siblings without ASD (LR-noASD) and had lower mean receptive and expressive language scores. Analyses of IBIS data corroborated that language delay, specifically receptive language delay, was more frequent in the HR-noASD (n = 235) versus LR-noASD group (n = 115). IBIS language scores were continuously and unimodally distributed, with a pathological shift towards decreased language function in HR-noASD siblings. The elevated inherited risk for ASD was associated with lower receptive and expressive language scores when controlling for sociodemographic factors. For receptive but not expressive language, the effect of risk group remained significant even when controlling for nonverbal cognition.

Conclusions: Greater frequency of language delay and a lower distribution of language scores in high-risk, unaffected toddler-aged siblings support decreased early language ability as an endophenotype for ASD, with a more pronounced effect for receptive versus expressive language. Further characterization of language development is warranted to refine genetic investigations of ASD and to elucidate factors influencing the progression of core autistic traits and related symptoms.

Citing Articles

Change in aperiodic activity over first year of life is associated with later autism diagnosis and 18-month language development in infants with family history of autism.

Wilkinson C, Chung H, Dave A, Tager-Flusberg H, Nelson C medRxiv. 2025; .

PMID: 39763568 PMC: 11702732. DOI: 10.1101/2024.12.15.24319061.


Considerations for Measuring Caregiver Talk in Interactions With Infants at Elevated and Population-Level Likelihood for Autism: Deriving Stable Estimates.

Bottema-Beutel K, Guo R, Braun C, Dunham-Carr K, Markfeld J, Pulliam G J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2024; 68(1):234-247.

PMID: 39680788 PMC: 11842079. DOI: 10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00312.


Links Between Early Prelinguistic Communication and Later Expressive Language in Toddlers With Autistic and Non-Autistic Siblings.

Markfeld J, Kiemel Z, Santapuram P, Bordman S, Pulliam G, Clark S J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2024; 68(1):178-192.

PMID: 39637288 PMC: 11842043. DOI: 10.1044/2024_JSLHR-23-00794.


Factors associated with receptive and expressive language in autistic children and siblings: A systematic review.

Mues M, Schaubroeck S, Demurie E, Roeyers H Autism Dev Lang Impair. 2024; 9:23969415241253554.

PMID: 38746787 PMC: 11092550. DOI: 10.1177/23969415241253554.


Atypical functional connectivity between the amygdala and visual, salience regions in infants with genetic liability for autism.

Liu J, Girault J, Nishino T, Shen M, Kim S, Burrows C Cereb Cortex. 2024; 34(13):30-39.

PMID: 38696599 PMC: 11065105. DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhae092.


References
1.
Bailey A, Palferman S, Heavey L, Le Couteur A . Autism: the phenotype in relatives. J Autism Dev Disord. 1998; 28(5):369-92. DOI: 10.1023/a:1026048320785. View

2.
Tomblin J, Hafeman L, OBrien M . Autism and autism risk in siblings of children with specific language impairment. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2003; 38(3):235-50. DOI: 10.1080/1368282031000086363. View

3.
Bishop D, Laws G, Adams C, Norbury C . High heritability of speech and language impairments in 6-year-old twins demonstrated using parent and teacher report. Behav Genet. 2006; 36(2):173-84. DOI: 10.1007/s10519-005-9020-0. View

4.
Duff F, Reen G, Plunkett K, Nation K . Do infant vocabulary skills predict school-age language and literacy outcomes?. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2015; 56(8):848-56. PMC: 4674965. DOI: 10.1111/jcpp.12378. View

5.
Mitchell S, Brian J, Zwaigenbaum L, Roberts W, Szatmari P, Smith I . Early language and communication development of infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2006; 27(2 Suppl):S69-78. DOI: 10.1097/00004703-200604002-00004. View