» Articles » PMID: 29931656

Comparative Social Grooming Networks in Captive Chimpanzees and Bonobos

Overview
Journal Primates
Specialty Biology
Date 2018 Jun 23
PMID 29931656
Citations 7
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Despite similar dispersal patterns, models of Pan sociality emphasize sex differences in social bonding between the two species. Such disparities are attributed to hypothesized differences in environmental selective pressures that structure association patterns. However, recent research documents greater within-species variation in social bonds in both species. Here, we examine grooming networks in captive chimpanzees at the North Carolina Zoo, and captive bonobos at the Columbus Zoo. We hypothesized that male-female grooming relationships would be the strongest in both species, but that males and females of both species would not significantly differ between centrality, strength, or clustering. Via Mantel tests, we found that neither bonobos (t = - 0.070, r = - 0.009, two-tailed p = 0.942) nor chimpanzees (t = - 0.495, r = - 0.0939, two-tailed p = 0.6205) had significant differences in grooming between or within sexes. Neither species had significant sex differences in centrality, strength, or clustering. To account for idiosyncratic factors affecting grooming distribution, we examined the effect of origin, kinship, and group tenure on social network position. We found that wild-born bonobos exhibited greater eigenvector centrality (t = - 2.592, df = 9, p = 0.29) and strength (t = - 2.401; df = 9, p = 0.040), and group tenure was significantly correlated with strength (r = 0.608; N = 11, p - 0 = 0.47). None of these factors varied with social network position in chimpanzees. Our findings suggest that in captive settings, idiosyncratic factors related to individual history play a greater role in structuring social networks. Such variation may point to the behavioral flexibility inherent in fission-fusion networks, and mirror between-site variation found in wild chimpanzees. However, some idiosyncratic factors shaping captive networks may be an artifact of captivity.

Citing Articles

Multi-group analysis of grooming network position in a highly social primate.

Torfs J, Stevens J, Verspeek J, Lameris D, Guery J, Eens M PLoS One. 2023; 18(4):e0284361.

PMID: 37099520 PMC: 10132689. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0284361.


The neuroecology of the water-to-land transition and the evolution of the vertebrate brain.

MacIver M, Finlay B Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2021; 377(1844):20200523.

PMID: 34957852 PMC: 8710882. DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2020.0523.


Cofeeding tolerance in chimpanzees depends on group composition: a longitudinal study across four communities.

DeTroy S, Ross C, Cronin K, van Leeuwen E, Haun D iScience. 2021; 24(3):102175.

PMID: 33733060 PMC: 7940988. DOI: 10.1016/j.isci.2021.102175.


Assessing the sociability of former pet and entertainment chimpanzees by using multiplex networks.

Crailsheim D, Romani T, Llorente M, Kalcher-Sommersguter E Sci Rep. 2020; 10(1):20969.

PMID: 33262388 PMC: 7708499. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-020-77950-x.


Spatial planning with long visual range benefits escape from visual predators in complex naturalistic environments.

Mugan U, MacIver M Nat Commun. 2020; 11(1):3057.

PMID: 32546681 PMC: 7298009. DOI: 10.1038/s41467-020-16102-1.


References
1.
Parish A, de Waal F . The other "closest living relative". How bonobos (Pan paniscus) challenge traditional assumptions about females, dominance, intra- and intersexual interactions, and hominid evolution. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2000; 907:97-113. View

2.
Hohmann , Fruth . Use and function of genital contacts among female bonobos. Anim Behav. 2000; 60(1):107-120. DOI: 10.1006/anbe.2000.1451. View

3.
Newman M . The structure of scientific collaboration networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001; 98(2):404-9. PMC: 14598. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.404. View

4.
Hashimoto C, Suzuki S, Takenoshita Y, Yamagiwa J, Basabose A, Furuichi T . How fruit abundance affects the chimpanzee party size: a comparison between four study sites. Primates. 2003; 44(2):77-81. DOI: 10.1007/s10329-002-0026-4. View

5.
Barrat A, Barthelemy M, Pastor-Satorras R, Vespignani A . The architecture of complex weighted networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004; 101(11):3747-52. PMC: 374315. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0400087101. View