» Articles » PMID: 27445925

3-Year-Old Children Selectively Generalize Object Functions Following a Demonstration from a Linguistic In-group Member: Evidence from the Phenomenon of Scale Error

Overview
Journal Front Psychol
Date 2016 Jul 23
PMID 27445925
Citations 5
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

The present study investigated 3-year-old children's learning processes about object functions. We built on children's tendency to commit scale errors with tools to explore whether they would selectively endorse object functions from a linguistic in-group over an out-group model. Participants (n = 37) were presented with different object sets, and a model speaking either in their native or a foreign language demonstrated how to use the presented tools. In the test phase, children received the object sets with two modifications: the original tool was replaced by one that was too big to achieve the goal but was otherwise identical, and another tool was added to the set that looked different but was appropriately scaled for goal attainment. Children in the Native language condition were significantly more likely to commit scale errors - that is, choose the over-sized tool - than children in the Foreign language condition (48 vs. 30%). We propose that these results provide insight into the characteristics of human-specific learning processes by showing that children are more likely to generalize object functions to a category of artifacts following a demonstration from an in-group member.

Citing Articles

Young children expect pretend object identities to be known only by their partners in joint pretence.

Andrasi K, Schvajda R, Kiraly I Br J Dev Psychol. 2022; 40(3):398-409.

PMID: 35531952 PMC: 9545026. DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12417.


Two-Year-Old Children Expect Native, but Not Foreign Speakers to Use the Same Tool for the Same Purpose.

Peto R, Olah K, Kiraly I Front Psychol. 2021; 12:675595.

PMID: 34484033 PMC: 8416162. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.675595.


Social categorization based on permanent versus transient visual traits in neurotypical children and children with autism spectrum disorder.

Kiss O, Olah K, Feher L, Topal J Sci Rep. 2021; 11(1):6549.

PMID: 33753814 PMC: 7985514. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-021-85924-w.


Young Children Selectively Imitate Models Conforming to Social Norms.

Olah K, Kiraly I Front Psychol. 2019; 10:1399.

PMID: 31293474 PMC: 6606772. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01399.


Similarity between an unfamiliar human and the owner affects dogs' preference for human partner when responding to an unsolvable problem.

Kiss O, Kovacs K, Szantho F, Topal J Learn Behav. 2018; 46(4):430-441.

PMID: 30022446 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-018-0337-y.

References
1.
Kinzler K, Spelke E . Core systems in human cognition. Prog Brain Res. 2007; 164:257-64. DOI: 10.1016/S0079-6123(07)64014-X. View

2.
Kinzler K, Dupoux E, Spelke E . The native language of social cognition. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007; 104(30):12577-80. PMC: 1941511. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0705345104. View

3.
Kinzler K, Dupoux E, Spelke E . "Native" Objects and Collaborators: Infants' Object Choices and Acts of Giving Reflect Favor for Native Over Foreign Speakers. J Cogn Dev. 2012; 13(1):67-81. PMC: 3478775. DOI: 10.1080/15248372.2011.567200. View

4.
Glover S . Separate visual representations in the planning and control of action. Behav Brain Sci. 2004; 27(1):3-24. DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x04000020. View

5.
DeLoache J, Uttal D, Rosengren K . Scale errors offer evidence for a perception-action dissociation early in life. Science. 2004; 304(5673):1027-9. DOI: 10.1126/science.1093567. View