» Articles » PMID: 26557059

Looming Sensitive Cortical Regions Without V1 Input: Evidence from a Patient with Bilateral Cortical Blindness

Overview
Date 2015 Nov 12
PMID 26557059
Citations 11
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Fast and automatic behavioral responses are required to avoid collision with an approaching stimulus. Accordingly, looming stimuli have been found to be highly salient and efficient attractors of attention due to the implication of potential collision and potential threat. Here, we address the question of whether looming motion is processed in the absence of any functional primary visual cortex and consequently without awareness. For this, we investigated a patient (TN) suffering from complete, bilateral damage to his primary visual cortex. Using an fMRI paradigm, we measured TN's brain activation during the presentation of looming, receding, rotating, and static point lights, of which he was unaware. When contrasted with other conditions, looming was found to produce bilateral activation of the middle temporal areas, as well as the superior temporal sulcus and inferior parietal lobe (IPL). The latter are generally thought to be involved in multisensory processing of motion in extrapersonal space, as well as attentional capture and saliency. No activity was found close to the lesioned V1 area. This demonstrates that looming motion is processed in the absence of awareness through direct subcortical projections to areas involved in multisensory processing of motion and saliency that bypass V1.

Citing Articles

On the bright side of blindsight. Considerations from new observations of awareness in a blindsight patient.

de Gelder B, Humphrey N, Pegna A Cereb Cortex. 2024; 35(1):42-48.

PMID: 39604004 PMC: 11712267. DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhae456.


Human subcortical pathways automatically detect collision trajectory without attention and awareness.

Guo F, Zou J, Wang Y, Fang B, Zhou H, Wang D PLoS Biol. 2024; 22(1):e3002375.

PMID: 38236815 PMC: 10795999. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3002375.


Convolutional neural networks for vision neuroscience: significance, developments, and outstanding issues.

Celeghin A, Borriero A, Orsenigo D, Diano M, Mendez Guerrero C, Perotti A Front Comput Neurosci. 2023; 17:1153572.

PMID: 37485400 PMC: 10359983. DOI: 10.3389/fncom.2023.1153572.


Exogenous Attention to Emotional Stimuli Presenting Realistic (3D) Looming Motion.

Fernandez-Folgueiras U, Hernandez-Lorca M, Mendez-Bertolo C, Alvarez F, Gimenez-Fernandez T, Carretie L Brain Topogr. 2022; 35(5-6):599-612.

PMID: 35933532 PMC: 9684254. DOI: 10.1007/s10548-022-00909-w.


Shallow neural networks trained to detect collisions recover features of visual loom-selective neurons.

Zhou B, Li Z, Kim S, Lafferty J, Clark D Elife. 2022; 11.

PMID: 35023828 PMC: 8849349. DOI: 10.7554/eLife.72067.


References
1.
Gottlieb J, Kusunoki M, Goldberg M . The representation of visual salience in monkey parietal cortex. Nature. 1998; 391(6666):481-4. DOI: 10.1038/35135. View

2.
Vogt B, Vogt L, Laureys S . Cytology and functionally correlated circuits of human posterior cingulate areas. Neuroimage. 2005; 29(2):452-66. PMC: 2649771. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.048. View

3.
Leh S, Johansen-Berg H, Ptito A . Unconscious vision: new insights into the neuronal correlate of blindsight using diffusion tractography. Brain. 2006; 129(Pt 7):1822-32. DOI: 10.1093/brain/awl111. View

4.
King S, Dykeman C, Redgrave P, Dean P . Use of a distracting task to obtain defensive head movements to looming visual stimuli by human adults in a laboratory setting. Perception. 1992; 21(2):245-59. DOI: 10.1068/p210245. View

5.
Barbur J, Watson J, Frackowiak R, Zeki S . Conscious visual perception without V1. Brain. 1993; 116 ( Pt 6):1293-302. DOI: 10.1093/brain/116.6.1293. View