» Articles » PMID: 26300841

To Transform or Not to Transform: Using Generalized Linear Mixed Models to Analyse Reaction Time Data

Overview
Journal Front Psychol
Date 2015 Aug 25
PMID 26300841
Citations 232
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

Linear mixed-effect models (LMMs) are being increasingly widely used in psychology to analyse multi-level research designs. This feature allows LMMs to address some of the problems identified by Speelman and McGann (2013) about the use of mean data, because they do not average across individual responses. However, recent guidelines for using LMM to analyse skewed reaction time (RT) data collected in many cognitive psychological studies recommend the application of non-linear transformations to satisfy assumptions of normality. Uncritical adoption of this recommendation has important theoretical implications which can yield misleading conclusions. For example, Balota et al. (2013) showed that analyses of raw RT produced additive effects of word frequency and stimulus quality on word identification, which conflicted with the interactive effects observed in analyses of transformed RT. Generalized linear mixed-effect models (GLMM) provide a solution to this problem by satisfying normality assumptions without the need for transformation. This allows differences between individuals to be properly assessed, using the metric most appropriate to the researcher's theoretical context. We outline the major theoretical decisions involved in specifying a GLMM, and illustrate them by reanalysing Balota et al.'s datasets. We then consider the broader benefits of using GLMM to investigate individual differences.

Citing Articles

Perceptual adaptation to dysarthric speech is modulated by concurrent phonological processing: A dual task study.

Adank P, Wang H, Hepworth T, Borrie S J Acoust Soc Am. 2025; 157(3):1598-1611.

PMID: 40063084 PMC: 11905114. DOI: 10.1121/10.0035883.


Working memory capacity moderates the effect of hearing aid experience on phonological processing performance.

Ning R, Holmer E, Danielsson H, Signoret C, Micula A, Ronnberg J Front Neurosci. 2025; 19:1519934.

PMID: 40040847 PMC: 11876422. DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2025.1519934.


Learning of the mean, but not variance, of color distributions cues target location probability.

Blonde P, Hansmann-Roth S, Pascucci D, Kristjansson A Sci Rep. 2025; 15(1):7591.

PMID: 40038258 PMC: 11880396. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-024-84750-0.


Neural mechanisms underlying the after-effects of repetitive paired-pulse TMS with Β tACS on the human primary motor cortex.

Nakazono H, Ogata K, Mitsutake T, Takeda A, Yamada E, Tobimatsu S Sci Rep. 2025; 15(1):7286.

PMID: 40025222 PMC: 11873315. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-025-92444-4.


Co-representation breaks down beyond the dyad in UK adults.

Milward S, Whitehouse J PLoS One. 2025; 20(2):e0318545.

PMID: 39999116 PMC: 11856543. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0318545.


References
1.
Borowsky R, Besner D . Visual word recognition: a multistage activation model. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1993; 19(4):813-40. DOI: 10.1037//0278-7393.19.4.813. View

2.
Ratcliff R, Rouder J . A diffusion model account of masking in two-choice letter identification. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2000; 26(1):127-40. DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.26.1.127. View

3.
Rumelhart D, McClelland J . An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 2. The contextual enhancement effect and some tests and extensions of the model. Psychol Rev. 1982; 89(1):60-94. View

4.
Wagenmakers E, Brown S . On the linear relation between the mean and the standard deviation of a response time distribution. Psychol Rev. 2007; 114(3):830-41. DOI: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.3.830. View

5.
Yap M, Tse C, Balota D . Individual differences in the joint effects of semantic priming and word frequency: The role of lexical integrity. J Mem Lang. 2010; 61(3):303. PMC: 2818444. DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2009.07.001. View