» Articles » PMID: 26284004

Rewards Modulate Saccade Latency but Not Exogenous Spatial Attention

Overview
Journal Front Psychol
Date 2015 Aug 19
PMID 26284004
Citations 11
Authors
Affiliations
Soon will be listed here.
Abstract

The eye movement system is sensitive to reward. However, whilst the eye movement system is extremely flexible, the extent to which changes to oculomotor behavior induced by reward paradigms persist beyond the training period or transfer to other oculomotor tasks is unclear. To address these issues we examined the effects of presenting feedback that represented small monetary rewards to spatial locations on the latency of saccadic eye movements, the time-course of learning and extinction of the effects of rewarding saccades on exogenous spatial attention and oculomotor inhibition of return. Reward feedback produced a relative facilitation of saccadic latency in a stimulus driven saccade task which persisted for three blocks of extinction trials. However, this hemifield-specific effect failed to transfer to peripheral cueing tasks. We conclude that rewarding specific spatial locations is unlikely to induce long-term, systemic changes to the human oculomotor or attention systems.

Citing Articles

Motivation by reward jointly improves speed and accuracy, whereas task-relevance and meaningful images do not.

Wolf C, Lappe M Atten Percept Psychophys. 2022; 85(3):930-948.

PMID: 36289140 PMC: 10066132. DOI: 10.3758/s13414-022-02587-z.


Vision as oculomotor reward: cognitive contributions to the dynamic control of saccadic eye movements.

Wolf C, Lappe M Cogn Neurodyn. 2021; 15(4):547-568.

PMID: 34367360 PMC: 8286912. DOI: 10.1007/s11571-020-09661-y.


Hypomania and saccadic changes in Parkinson's disease: influence of D2 and D3 dopaminergic signalling.

Pelzer E, Dillenburger B, Grundmann S, Iliaev V, Aschenberg S, Melzer C NPJ Parkinsons Dis. 2020; 6:5.

PMID: 31970287 PMC: 6969176. DOI: 10.1038/s41531-019-0107-3.


On the link between attentional search and the oculomotor system: Is preattentive search restricted to the range of eye movements?.

Casteau S, Smith D Atten Percept Psychophys. 2020; 82(2):518-532.

PMID: 31942703 PMC: 7246251. DOI: 10.3758/s13414-019-01949-4.


Reward learning biases the direction of saccades.

Liao M, Anderson B Cognition. 2019; 196:104145.

PMID: 31770659 PMC: 7028500. DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104145.


References
1.
MacLean G, Klein R, Hilchey M . Does oculomotor readiness mediate exogenous capture of visual attention?. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2015; 41(5):1260-70. DOI: 10.1037/xhp0000064. View

2.
Ikeda T, Hikosaka O . Positive and negative modulation of motor response in primate superior colliculus by reward expectation. J Neurophysiol. 2007; 98(6):3163-70. DOI: 10.1152/jn.00975.2007. View

3.
Balleine B, Dickinson A . Goal-directed instrumental action: contingency and incentive learning and their cortical substrates. Neuropharmacology. 1998; 37(4-5):407-19. DOI: 10.1016/s0028-3908(98)00033-1. View

4.
Ikkai A, Curtis C . Common neural mechanisms supporting spatial working memory, attention and motor intention. Neuropsychologia. 2010; 49(6):1428-34. PMC: 3081523. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.020. View

5.
Smith D, Ball K, Ellison A, Schenk T . Deficits of reflexive attention induced by abduction of the eye. Neuropsychologia. 2009; 48(5):1269-76. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.12.028. View